A systematic exploration of attentional load effects on the C1 ERP component.

The C1 ERP component reflects the earliest visual processing in V1. However, it remains debated whether attentional load can influence it or not. We conducted two EEG experiments to investigate the effect of attentional load on the C1. Task difficulty was manipulated at fixation using an oddball detection task that was either easy (low load) or difficult (high load), while the distractor was presented in the upper visual field (UVF) to score the C1. In Experiment 1, we used a block design and the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between the central stimulus and the peripheral distractor was either short or long. In Experiment 2, task difficulty was manipulated on a trial-by-trial basis using a visual cue, and the peripheral distractor was presented either before or after the central stimulus. The results showed that the C1 was larger in the high compared to the low load condition irrespective of SOA in Experiment 1. In Experiment 2, no significant load modulation of the C1 was observed. However, we found that the contingent negative variation (CNV) was larger in the low compared to the high load condition. Moreover, the C1 was larger when the peripheral distractor was presented after than before the central stimulus. Combined together, these results suggest that different top-down control processes can influence the initial feedforward stage of visual processing in V1 captured by the C1 ERP component.

[1]  K. Rauss,et al.  Retinotopically specific effects of attention on human early visual cortex activity. , 2022, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[2]  G. Pourtois,et al.  Top-Down Modulation of Early Visual Processing in V1: Dissociable Neurophysiological Effects of Spatial Attention, Attentional Load and Task-Relevance. , 2021, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  S. Luck,et al.  Progress toward resolving the attentional capture debate , 2020, Visual cognition.

[4]  K. Rauss,et al.  Anatomic and functional asymmetries interactively shape human early visual cortex responses , 2020, Journal of vision.

[5]  Galia Avidan,et al.  Rapid forgetting of faces in congenital prosopagnosia , 2020, Cortex.

[6]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  A Tutorial on Conducting and Interpreting a Bayesian ANOVA in JASP , 2020, L’Année psychologique.

[7]  S. Fu ‘Tricks’ for revealing potential attentional modulations on the C1 component , 2018, Cognitive neuroscience.

[8]  A. Proverbio,et al.  Endogenous attention to object features modulates the ERP C1 component , 2018, Cognitive neuroscience.

[9]  Michael A. Pitts,et al.  Does spatial attention modulate the earliest component of the visual evoked potential? , 2018, Cognitive neuroscience.

[10]  S. Slotnick The experimental parameters that affect attentional modulation of the ERP C1 component , 2018, Cognitive neuroscience.

[11]  Anna C. Nobre,et al.  Anticipated moments: temporal structure in attention , 2017, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  Gilles Pourtois,et al.  Motivational Salience Modulates Early Visual Cortex Responses across Task Sets* , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  G. Pourtois,et al.  Someone’s lurking in the dark: The role of state anxiety on attention deployment to threat-related stimuli , 2017, Biological Psychology.

[14]  G. Pourtois,et al.  Independent effects of motivation and spatial attention in the human visual cortex , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[15]  Edward Awh,et al.  The contralateral delay activity as a neural measure of visual working memory , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[16]  S. Luck,et al.  How inappropriate high-pass filters can produce artifactual effects and incorrect conclusions in ERP studies of language and cognition. , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[17]  D. Besner,et al.  A Resource-Control Account of Sustained Attention , 2015, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[18]  Gilles Pourtois,et al.  Electrical neuroimaging reveals content-specific effects of threat in primary visual cortex and fronto-parietal attentional networks , 2014, NeuroImage.

[19]  Zhe Qu,et al.  Earliest stages of visual cortical processing are not modified by attentional load , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[20]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Attentional Mechanisms of Distractor Suppression , 2014 .

[21]  Sabine Kastner,et al.  The Oxford Handbook of Attention , 2014 .

[22]  Charles E. Schroeder,et al.  The cruciform model of striate generation of the early VEP, re-illustrated, not revoked: A reply to Ales et al. (2013) , 2013, NeuroImage.

[23]  E. Vogel,et al.  Visual working memory capacity: from psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  Justin M. Ales,et al.  On determining the intracranial sources of visual evoked potentials from scalp topography: A reply to Kelly et al. (this issue) , 2013, NeuroImage.

[25]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Attentional load is not a critical factor for eliciting C1 attentional effect – A reply to Rauss, Pourtois, Vuilleumier, and Schwartz , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[26]  Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al.  Attention Reverses the Effect of Prediction in Silencing Sensory Signals , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al.  Less Is More: Expectation Sharpens Representations in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[29]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Towards the utilization of EEG as a brain imaging tool , 2012, NeuroImage.

[30]  Gilles Pourtois,et al.  State-dependent attention modulation of human primary visual cortex: A high density ERP study , 2012, NeuroImage.

[31]  Gilles Pourtois,et al.  Effects of attentional load on early visual processing depend on stimulus timing , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[32]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Coregistration of eye movements and EEG in natural reading: analyses and review. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  M. Mather,et al.  Arousal-Biased Competition in Perception and Memory , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[34]  J. Theeuwes Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[35]  Justin M. Ales,et al.  V1 is not uniquely identified by polarity reversals of responses to upper and lower visual field stimuli , 2010, NeuroImage.

[36]  R. B. Reilly,et al.  FASTER: Fully Automated Statistical Thresholding for EEG artifact Rejection , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[37]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Dissociation of visual C1 and P1 components as a function of attentional load: An event-related potential study , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[38]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Early interaction between perceptual load and involuntary attention: An event-related potential study , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[39]  Yan Wang,et al.  Perceptual load interacts with involuntary attention at early processing stages: Event-related potential studies , 2009, NeuroImage.

[40]  R. Adorni,et al.  C1 and P1 visual responses to words are enhanced by attention to orthographic vs. lexical properties , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[41]  G. Pourtois,et al.  Attentional load modifies early activity in human primary visual cortex , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[42]  C. Spence,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology the Role of Working Memory in Tactile Selective Attention , 2022 .

[43]  William S Helton,et al.  Impulsive responding and the sustained attention to response task , 2009, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[44]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Spatial attention modulates initial afferent activity in human primary visual cortex. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  Harvey A Swadlow,et al.  Task difficulty modulates the activity of specific neuronal populations in primary visual cortex , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  G. Pourtois,et al.  Effects of perceptual learning on primary visual cortex activity in humans , 2008, Vision Research.

[47]  A. Nobre,et al.  The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[48]  C. Carlson,et al.  Performance on a neurocognitive measure of alerting differentiates ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes: a preliminary report. , 2007, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[49]  B. Bahrami,et al.  Attentional Load Modulates Responses of Human Primary Visual Cortex to Invisible Stimuli , 2007, Current Biology.

[50]  Marty G Woldorff,et al.  Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention , 2007, PLoS biology.

[51]  Jin Fan,et al.  Development of attentional networks: An fMRI study with children and adults , 2005, NeuroImage.

[52]  S. Kastner,et al.  Stimulus context modulates competition in human extrastriate cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  Jin Fan,et al.  The activation of attentional networks , 2005, NeuroImage.

[54]  R. Dolan,et al.  Attentional load and sensory competition in human vision: modulation of fMRI responses by load at fixation during task-irrelevant stimulation in the peripheral visual field. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  Randolph Blake,et al.  The role of temporal structure in human vision. , 2005, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[56]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Identification of the neural sources of the pattern-reversal VEP , 2005, NeuroImage.

[57]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  M. Pinsk,et al.  Push-pull mechanism of selective attention in human extrastriate cortex. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Popout modulates focal attention in the primary visual cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[60]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2004, Nature.

[61]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[62]  Antigona Martínez,et al.  Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[63]  M. Pinsk,et al.  Attention modulates responses in the human lateral geniculate nucleus , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[64]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[65]  C. Frith,et al.  The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention , 2001, Science.

[66]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  The Effects of Signal Salience and Caffeine on Performance, Workload, and Stress in an Abbreviated Vigilance Task , 2000, Hum. Factors.

[67]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[68]  D. Heeger,et al.  Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[69]  D. Somers,et al.  Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[70]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. , 1999 .

[71]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[72]  C D Frith,et al.  Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.

[73]  A. Pecchinenda The Affective Significance of Skin Conductance Activity During a Difficult Problem-solving Task , 1996 .

[74]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation , 1995, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[75]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses , 1994 .

[76]  B. C. Motter Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[77]  J. Brehm,et al.  The intensity of motivation. , 1989, Annual review of psychology.

[78]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Attention and Arousal , 1982 .

[79]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[80]  J J Tecce,et al.  Attention Reduction and Suppressed Direct-Current Potentials in the Human Brain , 1969, Science.

[81]  W. Walter,et al.  Contingent Negative Variation : An Electric Sign of Sensori-Motor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain , 1964, Nature.

[82]  N. Mackworth The Breakdown of Vigilance during Prolonged Visual Search 1 , 1948 .