Force-on-Force Handgun Practice

Objective: The aim of this study was to explore human factors aspects of reality-based “force-on-force” (FoF) handgun practice through a within-subjects field experiment that assessed subjective stress measurements, biomarker regulation, performance outcomes, and behavioral adaptations. Background: FoF handgun practice is a recent training asset for armed officers whereby dynamic opponents may act, react, and even retaliate with specially designed marker ammunition. Predesigned scenarios enable trainees to practice in a simulated real-life environment. Method: A sample of experienced military personnel (N = 20) ran a handgun workshop in two conditions: FoF practice and traditional cardboard-target practice. Intra-individual assessments included anticipated distress, subjective stress, salivary alpha-amylase (sAA), shooting accuracy, and directly observable training seriousness. Results: Compared with the standard cardboard practice condition, FoF exposure caused significant increases in anticipatory distress, subjective stress, and sAA secretion. Furthermore, participants’ first encounter with FoF practice (vs. cardboard practice) substantially degraded their shooting performance and had a significant positive impact on the earnestness with which they approached their mission during the workshop. Conclusion: FoF practice is an effective training tool for armed officers because it simulates a realistic work environment by increasing task-specific stress such that it affects important outcomes of professional performance and leads to desirable behavioral changes during training. Application: Potential applications of this research include the introduction of biomarker assessments in human factors research and the design, based on reality-based practice, of effective training procedures for high-reliability professionals.

[1]  R T Wilkinson,et al.  Performance under stress. , 1975, Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[2]  U. Nater,et al.  Salivary alpha-amylase as a non-invasive biomarker for the sympathetic nervous system: Current state of research , 2009, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[3]  S. Levine,et al.  Stress: an historical perspective , 2005 .

[4]  Nicolas Rohleder,et al.  Acute stress responses in salivary alpha-amylase predict increases of plasma norepinephrine , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[5]  R. L. Marca,et al.  Stress-induced changes in human salivary alpha-amylase activity—associations with adrenergic activity , 2006, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[6]  Gregory B. Morrison,et al.  Police handgun qualification: practical measure or aimless activity? , 1998 .

[7]  R. Flin Selecting the right stuff: Personality and high-reliability occupations. , 2001 .

[8]  David M. Kaplan,et al.  A Validity Study of the Subjective Unit of Discomfort (SUD) Score. , 1995 .

[9]  Douglas A. Granger,et al.  Salivary flow and alpha-amylase: Collection technique, duration, and oral fluid type , 2010, Physiology & Behavior.

[10]  S Montague,et al.  Measuring stress. , 1980, Nursing.

[11]  R. Oudejans,et al.  Effects of anxiety on handgun shooting behavior of police officers: a pilot study , 2010, Anxiety, stress, and coping.

[12]  Tom Smeets,et al.  The Risk of Being Shot At: Stress, Cortisol Secretion, and Their Impact on Memory and Perceived Learning During Reality- Based Practice for Armed Officers , 2011 .

[13]  O. Wolf,et al.  Stress selectively and lastingly promotes learning of context-related high arousing information , 2009, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[14]  Robert Karasek,et al.  Healthy Work : Stress, Productivity, and the Reconstruction of Working Life , 1990 .

[15]  N. Pattyn,et al.  Visuo-spatial path learning, stress, and cortisol secretion following military cadets’ first parachute jump: the effect of increasing task complexity , 2011, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[16]  K. Vohs,et al.  Psychology as the Science of Self-reports and Finger Movements Whatever Happened to Actual Behavior? , 2022 .

[17]  Gerald A. Hudgens,et al.  Salivary α‐amylase as a measure of endogenous adrenergic activity , 1996 .

[18]  G A Hudgens,et al.  Hormonal responses to psychological stress in men preparing for skydiving. , 1997, The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism.

[19]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  A. Garde,et al.  Evaluation of salivary cortisol as a biomarker of self-reported mental stress in field studies , 2004 .

[21]  Steven M. Southwick,et al.  Stress-Induced Deficits in Working Memory and Visuo-Constructive Abilities in Special Operations Soldiers , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  K. Vohs,et al.  Psychology as the Science of Self-Reports and Finger Movements , 2010 .

[23]  Lorraine Hope,et al.  Witnesses in Action , 2012, Psychological science.

[24]  Tricia Z. King,et al.  Shooting behaviour: How working memory and negative emotionality influence police officer shoot decisions , 2009 .

[25]  E. Gordis,et al.  Salivary alpha-amylase in biobehavioral research: recent developments and applications. , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[26]  S. Folkman,et al.  Stress, appraisal, and coping , 1974 .

[27]  Raymond Niaura,et al.  Stress response and the adolescent transition: Performance versus peer rejection stressors , 2009, Development and Psychopathology.

[28]  G A Hudgens,et al.  Salivary alpha-amylase as a measure of endogenous adrenergic activity. , 1996, Clinical physiology.