Motor conflict in Stroop tasks: Direct evidence from single-trial electro-myography and electro-encephalography
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. J. Fridlund,et al. Guidelines for human electromyographic research. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[2] T. Braver,et al. Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Response Conflict : Effects of Response Modality and Processing Domain , 2022 .
[3] E. Donchin. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.
[4] E Donchin,et al. A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.
[5] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.
[6] Valéria Csépe,et al. The effect of numerical distance and stimulus probability on ERP components elicited by numerical incongruencies in mental addition. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[7] John Polich,et al. P300 and response time from a manual Stroop task , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[8] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[9] C. Eriksen,et al. Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] Cameron S. Carter,et al. Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: A functional MRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.
[11] E. Gratton,et al. A continuously variable frequency cross-correlation phase fluorometer with picosecond resolution. , 1983, Biophysical journal.
[12] Borís Burle,et al. Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis , 2002, Psychological research.
[13] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.
[14] M. Kutas,et al. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.
[15] G Mulder,et al. Selective response activation can begin before stimulus recognition is complete: a psychophysiological and error analysis of continuous flow. , 1990, Acta psychologica.
[16] W. Skrandies. Visual information processing: topography of brain electrical activity , 1995, Biological Psychology.
[17] C. Eriksen,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .
[18] C J Aine,et al. Hemispheric Differences in Event‐Related Potentials to Stroop Stimuli , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[19] N. Cohen,et al. The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[20] V. Csépe,et al. The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: an electro-encephalography study , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[21] R. West,et al. Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[22] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[23] Carmen M. Atkinson,et al. Event-related potentials to Stroop and reverse Stroop stimuli. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[24] C. Eriksen,et al. An electromyographic examination of response competition , 1985 .
[25] G. McCarthy,et al. Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.
[26] H. Masaki,et al. An electrophysiological study of the locus of the interference effect in a stimulus-response compatibility paradigm. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[27] S M McGill,et al. The importance of normalization in the interpretation of surface electromyography: a proof of principle. , 1999, Journal of manipulative and physiological therapeutics.
[28] M. Posner. Chronometric explorations of mind , 1978 .
[29] H. Mayberg,et al. An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[30] C W Eriksen,et al. Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[31] John Morton,et al. Selective Attention to Words and Colours , 1973 .
[32] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[33] Alan S. Brown,et al. Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .
[34] C Alain,et al. Event-related neural activity associated with the Stroop task. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[35] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .
[36] C. Liston,et al. Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.
[37] Valéria Csépe,et al. Electroencephalography effects to semantic and non-semantic mismatch in properties of visually presented single-characters: The N2b and the N400 , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.
[38] D. Lehmann,et al. Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[39] L. Mulder,et al. Use of partial stimulus information in response processing. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] B. Kopell,et al. The Stroop effect: brain potentials localize the source of interference. , 1981, Science.
[41] Dénes Szücs,et al. Real-time Tracking of Motor Response Activation and Response Competition in a Stroop Task in Young Children: A Lateralized Readiness Potential Study , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[42] Hellmuth Obrig,et al. The fast optical signal—Robust or elusive when non-invasively measured in the human adult? , 2005, NeuroImage.
[43] H Egeth,et al. Verbal interference with encoding in a perceptual classification task. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.
[44] F. Vidal,et al. Effect of the irrelevant location of the response signal on choice reaction time: an electromyographic study in humans. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[45] Avishai Henik,et al. The Brain Locus of Interaction between Number and Size: A Combined Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Event-related Potential Study , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[46] Monica Fabiani,et al. When in Doubt, Do it Both Ways: Brain Evidence of the Simultaneous Activation of Conflicting Motor Responses in a Spatial Stroop Task , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[47] Ardi Roelofs,et al. Anterior cingulate cortex activity can be independent of response conflict in Stroop-like tasks , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[48] T. Braver,et al. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain. , 2001, Cerebral Cortex.
[49] Frank Rösler,et al. N400 Effects Reflect Activation Spread During Retrieval of Arithmetic Facts , 1999 .
[50] Fruzsina Soltész,et al. Event-related potentials dissociate facilitation and interference effects in the numerical Stroop paradigm , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[51] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[52] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.
[53] M. Posner,et al. Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .
[54] Fruzsina Soltész,et al. The interaction of task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimulus features in the number/size congruency paradigm: An ERP study , 2008, Brain Research.
[55] J. Lannou,et al. The Stroop's test evokes a negative brain potential, the N400. , 1997, The International journal of neuroscience.