Conceptual Spaces for Cognitive Architectures: A Lingua Franca for Different Levels of Representation

During the last decades, many Cognitive Architectures (CAs) have been realized adopting different assumptions about the organization and the representation of their knowledge level. Some of them (e.g. SOAR (Laird, 2012)) adopt a classical symbolic approach, some (e.g. LEABRA O’Reilly and Munakata (2000)) are based on a purely connectionist model, while others (e.g. CLARION (Sun, 2006)) adopt a hybrid approach combining connectionist and symbolic representational levels. Additionally, some attempts (e.g. biSOAR) trying to extend the representational capacities of CAs by integrating diagrammatical representations and reasoning are also available (Kurup & Chandrasekaran, 2007). In this paper we propose a reflection on the role that Conceptual Spaces, a framework developed by Gardenfors (2000) more than fifteen years ago, can play in the current development of the Knowledge Level in Cognitive Systems and Architectures. In particular, we claim that Conceptual Spaces offer a lingua franca that allows to unify and generalize many aspects of the symbolic, sub-symbolic and diagrammatic approaches (by overcoming some of their typical problems) and to integrate them on a common ground. In doing so we extend and detail some of the arguments explored by Gardenfors (1997) for defending the need of a conceptual, intermediate, representation level between the symbolic and the sub-symbolic one. In particular we focus on the advantages offered by Conceptual Spaces (with respect to symbolic and sub-symbolic approaches) in dealing with the problem of compositionality of representations based on typicality traits. Additionally, we argue that Conceptual Spaces could offer a unifying framework for interpreting many kinds of diagrammatic and analogical representations. As a consequence, their adoption could also favor the integration of diagrammatical representation and reasoning in CAs.

[1]  Antonio Chella,et al.  A Cognitive Architecture for Music Perception Exploiting Conceptual Spaces , 2015 .

[2]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Advancing the rationality debate , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[3]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Deep Learning , 2015, Nature.

[4]  B. Malt,et al.  An on-line investigation of prototype and exemplar strategies in classification. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  P. Johnson-Laird How We Reason , 2006 .

[6]  Christian Lebiere,et al.  How Limited Systematicity Emerges , 2014 .

[7]  J. Fodor,et al.  Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis , 1988, Cognition.

[8]  Salvatore Gaglio,et al.  Symbols and Subsymbols for Representing Knowledge: A Catalogue Raisonné , 1989, IJCAI.

[9]  Shimon Edelman,et al.  Representation, similarity, and the chorus of prototypes , 1993, Minds and Machines.

[10]  R. Rosenfeld Nature , 2009, Otolaryngology--head and neck surgery : official journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery.

[11]  Anne Kuefer,et al.  The Case For Mental Imagery , 2016 .

[12]  Salvatore Gaglio,et al.  A general theoretical framework for designing cognitive architectures: Hybrid and meta-level architectures for BICA , 2012, BICA 2012.

[13]  Shun-ichi Amari,et al.  Methods of information geometry , 2000 .

[14]  N. Hari Narayanan,et al.  Diagrammatic Reasoning: Cognitive and Computational Perspectives , 1995 .

[15]  Trevor Bekolay,et al.  A Large-Scale Model of the Functioning Brain , 2012, Science.

[16]  Charles Kemp,et al.  Bayesian models of cognition , 2008 .

[17]  P. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference, and Consciousness , 1985 .

[18]  Jeffrey Pennington,et al.  GloVe: Global Vectors for Word Representation , 2014, EMNLP.

[19]  Salvatore Gaglio,et al.  A Cognitive Architecture for Artificial Vision , 1997, Artif. Intell..

[20]  E. Machery Doing without Concepts , 2009 .

[21]  Gualtiero Piccinini,et al.  Two Kinds of Concept: Implicit and Explicit , 2011, Dialogue.

[22]  Bolei Zhou,et al.  Object Detectors Emerge in Deep Scene CNNs , 2014, ICLR.

[23]  Dov M. Gabbay,et al.  Neural-Symbolic Cognitive Reasoning , 2008, Cognitive Technologies.

[24]  Daniele P. Radicioni,et al.  A Common-Sense Conceptual Categorization System Integrating Heterogeneous Proxytypes and the Dual Process of Reasoning , 2015, IJCAI.

[25]  Christian Balkenius Lund Are There Dimensions in the Brain ? , 1999 .

[26]  Daniele P. Radicioni,et al.  Dual PECCS: a cognitive system for conceptual representation and categorization , 2017, J. Exp. Theor. Artif. Intell..

[27]  M. Frixione,et al.  Dealing with Concepts: From Cognitive Psychology to Knowledge Representation , 2013 .

[28]  Chris Eliasmith,et al.  Neural Engineering: Computation, Representation, and Dynamics in Neurobiological Systems , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks.

[29]  Antonio Chella,et al.  Towards a Cognitive Architecture for Music Perception , 2013, AIC@AI*IA.

[30]  Peter Gärdenfors,et al.  Reasoning about Categories in Conceptual Spaces , 2001, IJCAI.

[31]  Chris Eliasmith,et al.  Biologically Plausible, Human-scale Knowledge Representation , 2016, CogSci.

[32]  N. Foo Conceptual Spaces—The Geometry of Thought , 2022 .

[33]  John R Anderson,et al.  An integrated theory of the mind. , 2004, Psychological review.

[34]  Gianni Vercelli,et al.  Diagrammatic reasoning for planning and intelligent control , 2001 .

[35]  Finn V. Jensen,et al.  Bayesian Networks and Decision Graphs , 2001, Statistics for Engineering and Information Science.

[36]  Aaron Sloman,et al.  How Can We Reduce the Gulf between Artificial and Natural Intelligence? , 2014, AIC.

[37]  Ron Sun,et al.  Cognition and Multi-Agent Interaction: The CLARION Cognitive Architecture: Extending Cognitive Modeling to Social Simulation , 2005 .

[38]  B. Rundle Representations: Philosophical Essays on the Foundations of Cognitive Science , 1984 .

[39]  E. Rosch Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories. , 1975 .

[40]  Peter Gärdenfors,et al.  Applications of conceptual spaces : the case for geometric knowledge representation , 2015 .

[41]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Mastering the game of Go with deep neural networks and tree search , 2016, Nature.

[42]  Atsuyuki Okabe,et al.  Spatial Tessellations: Concepts and Applications of Voronoi Diagrams, Second Edition , 2000, Wiley Series in Probability and Mathematical Statistics.

[43]  K. Jellinger,et al.  In two minds: Dual processes and beyond. , 2009 .

[44]  G. Murphy,et al.  The Big Book of Concepts , 2002 .

[45]  Umberto Straccia,et al.  Reasoning within Fuzzy Description Logics , 2011, J. Artif. Intell. Res..

[46]  Ernest Davis,et al.  Commonsense reasoning and commonsense knowledge in artificial intelligence , 2015, Commun. ACM.

[47]  Giovanni Pilato,et al.  Artwork creation by a cognitive architecture integrating computational creativity and dual process approaches , 2016, BICA 2016.

[48]  Jeffrey Dean,et al.  Distributed Representations of Words and Phrases and their Compositionality , 2013, NIPS.

[49]  Laura Giordano,et al.  A non-monotonic Description Logic for reasoning about typicality , 2013, Artif. Intell..

[50]  L. Squire,et al.  Learning about categories in the absence of memory. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[51]  D. Kahneman Thinking, Fast and Slow , 2011 .

[52]  Christian Lebiere,et al.  How Limited Systematicity Emerges: A Computational Cognitive Neuroscience Approach (Author's Manuscript) , 2014 .

[53]  Atsuyuki Okabe,et al.  Spatial Tessellations: Concepts and Applications of Voronoi Diagrams , 1992, Wiley Series in Probability and Mathematical Statistics.

[54]  John E. Laird,et al.  The Soar Cognitive Architecture , 2012 .

[55]  Antonio Lieto,et al.  Representing Concepts in Formal Ontologies: Compositionality vs. Typicality Effects" , 2013 .

[56]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience: Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain , 2000 .

[57]  Peter Gärdenfors,et al.  Symbolic, Conceptual and Subconceptual Representations , 1997 .

[58]  Salvatore Gaglio,et al.  An architecture for autonomous agents exploiting conceptual representations , 1998, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[59]  Jeffrey Dean,et al.  Efficient Estimation of Word Representations in Vector Space , 2013, ICLR.

[60]  Balakrishnan Chandrasekaran,et al.  Modeling Memories of Large-scale Space Using a Bimodal Cognitive Architecture , 2007 .

[61]  Allen Newell,et al.  Computer science as empirical inquiry: symbols and search , 1976, CACM.

[62]  Marcello Frixione,et al.  Representing Concepts in Artificial Systems: A Clash of Requirements , 2011 .

[63]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  On the adequacy of prototype theory as a theory of concepts , 1981, Cognition.

[64]  Salvatore Gaglio,et al.  Anchoring symbols to conceptual spaces: the case of dynamic scenarios , 2003, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[65]  Silvia Coradeschi,et al.  Perceptual anchoring via conceptual spaces , 2004, AAAI 2004.