Expert individuation of objects increases activation in the fusiform face area of children

The role of experience in the development of brain mechanisms for face recognition is intensely debated. Experience with subordinate- and individual-level classification of faces is thought, by some, to be foundational in the development of the specialization of face recognition. Studying children with extremely intense interests (EII) provides an opportunity to examine experience-related changes in non-face object recognition in a population where face expertise is not fully developed. Here, two groups of school-aged children -one group with an EII with Pokémon cards and another group of age-matched controls - underwent fMRI while viewing faces, Pokémon characters, Pokémon objects, and Digimon characters. Pokémon objects were non-character Pokémon cards that experts do not typically individuate during game play and trading. Neither experts nor controls had previous experience with Digimon characters. As expected, experts and controls showed equivalent activation in the fusiform face area (FFA) with face stimuli. As predicted by the expertise hypothesis, experts showed greater activation than controls with Pokémon characters, and showed greater activation with Pokémon characters than Pokémon objects. Experts and controls showed equivalent activation with Digimon characters. However, heightened activation with Digimon characters in both groups suggested that there are other strong influences on the activation of the FFA beyond stimulus characteristics, experience, and classification level. By demonstrating the important role of expertise, the findings are inconsistent with a purely face-specific account of FFA function. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of the effects of expertise and categorization level on activation in the FFA in a group of typically developing children.

[1]  P. Bolton,et al.  The Childhood Asperger Syndrome Test (CAST) , 2007, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[2]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Conditions for Facelike Expertise with Objects Becoming a Ziggerin Expert—but Which Type? , 2022 .

[3]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. , 2000, Archives of general psychiatry.

[4]  Beatriz Luna,et al.  Visual category-selectivity for faces, places and objects emerges along different developmental trajectories. , 2007, Developmental science.

[5]  George S. Cree,et al.  Perceptual and Conceptual Interactions in Object Recognition and Expertise , 2009 .

[6]  Jennifer M. D. Yoon,et al.  Differential development of the ventral visual cortex extends through adolescence , 2010 .

[7]  Piotr Jaśkowski,et al.  Facing facts: neuronal mechanisms of face perception. , 2008, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[8]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Qingyang Li,et al.  Emotional perception: Meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing , 2011, NeuroImage.

[10]  Cindy M. Bukach,et al.  Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Domain specificity in visual cortex. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex , 2006, NeuroImage.

[14]  Kalanit Grill-Spector,et al.  The improbable simplicity of the fusiform face area , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  What constrains the organization of the ventral temporal cortex? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  M. Tarr,et al.  Levels of categorization in visual recognition studied using functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Current Biology.

[17]  Ken Kelley,et al.  Author's Personal Copy the Development of Conceptual Interests in Young Children , 2022 .

[18]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  Marc G. Berman,et al.  Evaluating functional localizers: The case of the FFA , 2010, NeuroImage.

[20]  A. Ishai,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.

[21]  M. Tarr,et al.  Becoming a “Greeble” Expert: Exploring Mechanisms for Face Recognition , 1997, Vision Research.

[22]  K. James Sensori-motor experience leads to changes in visual processing in the developing brain. , 2010, Developmental science.

[23]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Divide and conquer: A defense of functional localizers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[24]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces? , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  J. Haxby,et al.  The distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  Auditory and Action Semantic Features Activate Sensory-Specific Perceptual Brain Regions , 2003, Current Biology.

[28]  Perceptual Expertise , 2009, Encyclopedia of Biometrics.

[29]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  The systemizing quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  Kathy E. Johnson,et al.  Factors associated with the early emergence of intense interests within conceptual domains , 2004 .

[31]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Empathy Quotient: An Investigation of Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and Normal Sex Differences , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[33]  M. Farah,et al.  What is "special" about face perception? , 1998, Psychological review.

[34]  R. Poldrack Region of interest analysis for fMRI. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[35]  J. Deloache,et al.  Planes, trains, automobiles--and tea sets: extremely intense interests in very young children. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[36]  Michael J. Tarr,et al.  Task-Specific Codes for Face Recognition: How they Shape the Neural Representation of Features for Detection and Individuation , 2008, PloS one.

[37]  G. Rhodes,et al.  Is the Fusiform Face Area Specialized for Faces, Individuation, or Expert Individuation? , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  M. Tarr,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area is Part of a Network that Processes Faces at the Individual Level , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Brain areas engaged during visual judgments by involuntary access to novel semantic information , 2004, Vision Research.

[41]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Children’s Empathy Quotient and Systemizing Quotient: Sex Differences in Typical Development and in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[42]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  fMRI activation of the fusiform gyrus and amygdala to cartoon characters but not to faces in a boy with autism , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Autism and the development of face processing , 2006, Clinical Neuroscience Research.