Investigating the ‘latent’ deficit hypothesis: Age at time of head injury, implicit and executive functions and behavioral insight

This study investigated the 'latent deficit' hypothesis in two groups of head-injured patients with predominantly frontal lesions, those injured prior to steep morphological and corresponding functional maturational periods for frontal networks (<or=age 25), and those injured>28 years. The latent deficit hypothesis proposes that early injuries produce enduring cognitive deficits manifest later in the lifespan with graver consequences for behavior than adult injuries, particularly after frontal pathology (Eslinger, Grattan, Damasio & Damasio, 1992). Implicit and executive deficits both contribute to behavioral insight after frontal head injury (Barker, Andrade, Romanowski, Morton, & Wasti, 2006). On the basis of morphological and behavioral data, we hypothesized that early injury would confer greater vulnerability to impairment on tasks associated with frontal regions than later injury. Patients completed experimental tasks of implicit cognition, executive function measures and the DEX measure of behavioral insight (Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome: Wilson, Alderman, Burgess, Emslie, & Evans, 1996). The Early Injury group were more impaired on implicit cognition tasks compared to controls that Late Injury patients. There were no marked group differences on most executive function measures. Executive ability only contributed to behavioral awareness in the Early Injury Group. Findings showed that age at injury moderates the relationship between executive and implicit cognition and behavioral insight and that early injuries result in long-standing deficits to functions associated with frontal regions partially supporting the latent deficit hypothesis.

[1]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Prefrontal lesions impair the implicit and explicit learning of sequences on visuomotor tasks , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  G. Huston The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. , 1987, The Journal of rheumatology.

[3]  Junghoon Kim,et al.  Executive Function and Self‐awareness of “Real‐world” Behavior and Attention Deficits Following Traumatic Brain Injury , 2005, The Journal of head trauma rehabilitation.

[4]  L. Barker,et al.  Implicit cognition is impaired and dissociable in a head-injured group with executive deficits , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  J. Ponsford,et al.  Assessment of executive dysfunction following traumatic brain injury: Comparison of the BADS with other clinical neuropsychological measures , 2005, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[6]  D. Drotar,et al.  Predicting Longitudinal Patterns of Functional Deficits in Children with Traumatic Brain Injury Many Studies Have Found Evidence of Deterioration in Behavioral Adjust- Ment across Time and Persistent Postinjury Behavior Problems In , 2022 .

[7]  P. Dockree,et al.  Awareness of deficits in traumatic brain injury: A multidimensional approach to assessing metacognitive knowledge and online-awareness , 2006, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[8]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Developmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal lobe functioning , 1991 .

[9]  D. Neary Lesion Analysis in Neuropsychology , 1990 .

[10]  Alexander Leemans,et al.  Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood , 2008, NeuroImage.

[11]  M. Hallett,et al.  Dynamic cortical involvement in implicit and explicit motor sequence learning. A PET study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  Tim Shallice,et al.  The Hayling and Brixton Tests , 1997 .

[13]  A. Toga,et al.  In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Tobias Sommer,et al.  Evidence of Developmental Differences in Implicit Sequence Learning: An fMRI Study of Children and Adults , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  J J Sacks,et al.  Pediatric head injuries and deaths from bicycling in the United States. , 1996, Pediatrics.

[16]  L. Barker,et al.  Impaired Implicit Cognition with Intact Executive Function After Extensive Bilateral Prefrontal Pathology: A Case Study , 2004, Neurocase.

[17]  Laura A Flashman,et al.  Lack of awareness and its impact in traumatic brain injury. , 2002, NeuroRehabilitation.

[18]  D. Tranel,et al.  Effects of Early Onset Brain Injury on the Development of Cognition and Behavior: Introduction to the Special Issue , 2000, Developmental neuropsychology.

[19]  T. Paus,et al.  Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  C. Mateer,et al.  Developmental impact of frontal lobe injury in middle childhood , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  C. Coulton,et al.  Interaction Effects in Multiple Regression , 1993 .

[22]  B. Hayes,et al.  The nature and development of nonverbal implicit memory. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  T. Novack Cognitive and Behavioral Rehabilitation from Neurobiology to Clinical Practice , 2005 .

[24]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[25]  小羽 俊士,et al.  Wisconsin card sorting test , 2013 .

[26]  K. O'Connor,et al.  Pediatricians' Reported Practices Regarding Developmental Screening: Do Guidelines Work? Do They Help? , 2005, Pediatrics.

[27]  M. Johnston,et al.  Plasticity in the developing brain: implications for rehabilitation. , 2009, Developmental disabilities research reviews.

[28]  J. Fleming,et al.  Convergent validity of self-awareness measures and their association with employment outcome in adults following acquired brain injury , 2005, Brain injury.

[29]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  Suzanne E. Welcome,et al.  Mapping cortical change across the human life span , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  T. Robinson,et al.  Brain Plasticity and Behavior , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[32]  Michael Weiner,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive self-appraisal in neurodegenerative disease , 2010, NeuroImage.

[33]  P. Burgess,et al.  The ecological validity of tests of executive function , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[34]  M. Kennard,et al.  RELATION OF AGE TO MOTOR IMPAIRMENT IN MAN AND IN SUBHUMAN PRIMATES , 1940 .

[35]  Henry Rice,et al.  Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology , 1985, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[36]  Paul W. Burgess,et al.  Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome , 1997 .

[37]  Ursula Kirk,et al.  Understanding awareness deficits following brain injury. , 2000, NeuroRehabilitation.

[38]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[40]  Uta Frith,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Processes in Social Cognition , 2008, Neuron.

[41]  A. Damasio The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[42]  J. H. Howard,et al.  Developmental differences in implicit learning of spatial context. , 2007, Neuropsychology.

[43]  A. David,et al.  Self-awareness after acquired and traumatic brain injury , 2006, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[44]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  Hillary C. M. Nelson The National Adult Reading Test , 1982 .

[46]  C. Boake,et al.  Characteristics of impaired awareness after traumatic brain injury , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[47]  Cynthia A. Riccio,et al.  Concurrent Validity of the Tower Tasks as Measures of Executive Function in Adults: A Meta-Analysis , 2009, Applied neuropsychology.

[48]  L. Steinberg,et al.  CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Risk Taking in Adolescence New Perspectives From Brain and Behavioral Science , 2022 .

[49]  P. Eslinger,et al.  Adolescent Neuropsychological Development After Early Right Prefrontal Cortex Damage , 2000, Developmental neuropsychology.

[50]  S. MacDonald,et al.  Self-awareness after traumatic brain injury: a comparison of measures and their relationship to executive functions. , 2003, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS.

[51]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Two Forms of Sequential Implicit Learning , 1997, Consciousness and Cognition.

[52]  Rachael D. Seidler,et al.  Spatial and symbolic implicit sequence learning in young and older adults , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  C. Reynolds,et al.  Wechsler memory scale-revised , 1988 .

[54]  J. Rosenfeld,et al.  Functional Plasticity or Vulnerability After Early Brain Injury? , 2005, Pediatrics.

[55]  G. Winocur,et al.  In Search of the Self: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1999 .

[56]  A R Damasio,et al.  Long-Term Sequelae of Prefrontal Cortex Damage Acquired in Early Childhood , 2000, Developmental neuropsychology.

[57]  J. Fleming,et al.  The Relative Importance of Metacognitive Skills, Emotional Status, and Executive Function in Psychosocial Adjustment Following Acquired Brain Injury , 2005, The Journal of head trauma rehabilitation.

[58]  Rachael Seidler,et al.  Degree of handedness affects intermanual transfer of skill learning , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[59]  T. Hart,et al.  A longitudinal study of awareness of deficit after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury , 2009, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[60]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Intuition: a social cognitive neuroscience approach. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[61]  T. Coughlan,et al.  Social adjustment after closed head injury: a further follow-up seven years after injury. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[62]  N. Edwards,et al.  Comparing the effects of stimulation and propofol infusion rate on implicit and explicit memory formation. , 2001, British journal of anaesthesia.

[63]  J. Ashe,et al.  Neural correlates of encoding and expression in implicit sequence learning , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[64]  Martial Van der Linden,et al.  Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[65]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[66]  M. Van der Linden,et al.  Implicit sequence learning in children. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[67]  J. J. Ryan,et al.  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III , 2001 .

[68]  Matcheri S. Keshavan,et al.  Insight and frontal cortical function in schizophrenia: A review , 2006, Schizophrenia Research.

[69]  Michael S Lewis-Beck,et al.  Sage university papers. Series Quantitative applications in the social sciences , 2008 .

[70]  L. Jiménez,et al.  Implicit Sequence Learning with Competing Explicit Cues , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[71]  C. Mateer,et al.  Effects of frontal lobe injury in childhood , 1991 .

[72]  R. Bode,et al.  Analysis of patient and proxy ratings on the Dysexecutive Questionnaire: an application of Rasch analysis , 2007, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.

[73]  E. Bernat,et al.  Brain Indices of Nonconscious Associative Learning , 1997, Consciousness and Cognition.

[74]  Cecil R. Reynolds,et al.  A Model of the Development of Frontal Lobe Functioning: Findings From a Meta-Analysis , 2005, Applied neuropsychology.

[75]  D. Stuss,et al.  Is there a dysexecutive syndrome? , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[76]  D L Schacter,et al.  Toward a cognitive neuropsychology of awareness: implicit knowledge and anosognosia. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[77]  M. Lezak Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd ed. , 1995 .

[78]  A. Benton,et al.  Developmental outcomes after early prefrontal cortex damage , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[79]  T. Ownsworth,et al.  An integrated biopsychosocial approach to understanding awareness deficits in Alzheimer's disease and brain injury , 2006, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[80]  P. Eslinger,et al.  Cognitive and social development in children with prefrontal cortex lesions. , 1997 .

[81]  W. Sturm,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment , 2007, Journal of Neurology.

[82]  Robert A. Bornstein,et al.  The Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised , 1990 .

[83]  Vicki A. Anderson,et al.  Development of Executive Functions Through Late Childhood and Adolescence in an Australian Sample , 2001, Developmental neuropsychology.

[84]  B. Dawant,et al.  Quantifying cerebral changes in adolescence with MRI and deformation based morphometry , 2008, Journal of magnetic resonance imaging : JMRI.

[85]  T. Paus Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[86]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[87]  J. Dougherty,et al.  Loss of awareness in Alzheimer’s disease (AD): Considerations of lack of insight and legal liabilities in the United States , 2005, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[88]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Neural correlates of self-evaluative accuracy after traumatic brain injury , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[89]  T. Ownsworth,et al.  Development and Standardization of the Self-Regulation Skills Interview (SRSI): A New Clinical Assessment Tool for Acquired Brain Injury , 2000, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[90]  S. Levine,et al.  Narrative skill in children with early unilateral brain injury: a possible limit to functional plasticity. , 2009, Developmental science.

[91]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[92]  M. Linden,et al.  Are central executive functions working in patients with focal frontal lesions? , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[93]  K. Barlow,et al.  Late Neurologic and Cognitive Sequelae of Inflicted Traumatic Brain Injury in Infancy , 2005, Pediatrics.

[94]  Bryan Kolb,et al.  Fundamentals of human neuropsychology, 3rd ed. , 1990 .

[95]  J. Andrade,et al.  Hidden covariation detection produces faster, not slower, social judgments. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[96]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. , 2006 .

[97]  M. Spencer-Smith,et al.  Childhood brain insult: can age at insult help us predict outcome? , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[98]  Conor V. Dolan,et al.  Source (or Part of the following Source): Type Article Title Age-related Change in Executive Function: Developmental Trends and a Latent Variable Analysis Author(s) Age-related Change in Executive Function: Developmental Trends and a Latent Variable Analysis , 2022 .

[99]  Linda Ewing-Cobbs,et al.  Late intellectual and academic outcomes following traumatic brain injury sustained during early childhood. , 2006, Journal of neurosurgery.

[100]  C. K. Hsiao,et al.  Performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Among Adolescents in Taiwan: Norms, Factorial Structure, and Relation to Schizotypy , 2000, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[101]  J. Biebuyck,et al.  BRITISH JOURNAL OF ANAESTHESIA , 2005 .