Distinct fMRI patterns colocalized in the cingulate cortex underlie the after-effects of cognitive control on pain

Demanding tasks can influence following behaviors but the underlying mechanisms remain largely unclear. In the present functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, we used multivariate pattern analyses (MVPA) to compare patterns of brain activity associated with pain in response to noxious stimuli administered after a task requiring cognitive control (Stroop) and evaluate their functional interaction based on a mediation analysis model. We found that performing a difficult cognitive task leads to subsequent increases in pain and pain-related multivariate responses across the brain and within the anterior mid-cingulate cortex (aMCC). Moreover, an aMCC pattern predictive of task performance was further reactivated during pain and predicted ensuing increases in pain-related brain responses. This suggests functional interactions between distinct but partly co-localized neural networks underlying executive control and pain. These findings offer a new perspective on the functional role of the cingulate cortex in pain and cognition and provide a promising framework to investigate dynamical interactions between partly overlapping brain networks.

[1]  K. Davis,et al.  A neurocognitive model of attention to pain: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence , 2009, PAIN®.

[2]  Paul W Frankland,et al.  A pain in the ACC , 2005, Molecular pain.

[3]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The dorsal anterior cingulate cortex is selective for pain: Results from large-scale reverse inference , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  M. Lindquist,et al.  An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. , 2013, The New England journal of medicine.

[5]  A. Ishai,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.

[6]  Pierre Rainville,et al.  Cerebral and Cerebrospinal Processes Underlying Counterirritation Analgesia , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Vladimir N. Vapnik,et al.  The Nature of Statistical Learning Theory , 2000, Statistics for Engineering and Information Science.

[8]  M. Botvinick,et al.  A labor/leisure tradeoff in cognitive control. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[9]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[10]  Ethan Kross,et al.  Separate neural representations for physical pain and social rejection , 2014, Nature Communications.

[11]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  A Multilab Preregistered Replication of the Ego-Depletion Effect , 2016, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[12]  E. Hirt,et al.  When perception is more than reality: the effects of perceived versus actual resource depletion on self-regulatory behavior. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  H. Critchley The human cortex responds to an interoceptive challenge. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  S. Sternberg,et al.  Separate modifiability, mental modules, and the use of pure and composite measures to reveal them. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[15]  Alison J. Wiggett,et al.  Patterns of fMRI Activity Dissociate Overlapping Functional Brain Areas that Respond to Biological Motion , 2006, Neuron.

[16]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  K. Vohs,et al.  The Strength Model of Self-Regulation: Conclusions From the Second Decade of Willpower Research , 2018, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[18]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  What Is Ego Depletion? Toward a Mechanistic Revision of the Resource Model of Self-Control , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[19]  S. Rauch,et al.  The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[20]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. , 1997, Science.

[21]  Noelle M. Nelson,et al.  Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: a limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  G. Crombez,et al.  Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  P. Rainville,et al.  After‐effects of cognitive control on pain , 2013, European journal of pain.

[24]  Martin A Lindquist,et al.  Quantifying cerebral contributions to pain beyond nociception , 2017, Nature Communications.

[25]  M. L. Wood,et al.  Functional MRI of pain- and attention-related activations in the human cingulate cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Elliot T. Berkman,et al.  The neuroscience of "ego depletion": How the brain can help us understand why self-control seems limited. , 2016 .

[27]  Gregory M. Walton,et al.  Ego Depletion—Is It All in Your Head? , 2010, Psychological science.

[28]  N. Bolger,et al.  Brain Mediators of Predictive Cue Effects on Perceived Pain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Gian Domenico Iannetti,et al.  Pain in the ACC? , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  M. Hagger,et al.  Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the value of control , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  P. Rainville,et al.  Dissection of perceptual, motor and autonomic components of brain activity evoked by noxious stimulation , 2010, PAIN.

[34]  R. Wright,et al.  Fatigue determination of inhibitory strength and control: A babe in a bath. , 2019, Motivation Science.

[35]  Rebecca G. Boswell,et al.  Self-Regulatory Depletion Enhances Neural Responses to Rewards and Impairs Top-Down Control , 2013, Psychological science.

[36]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[38]  T. Wager,et al.  Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task , 2010, PAIN®.

[39]  M. Chen,et al.  Increased sensitivity of , 1998 .

[40]  T. Heatherton,et al.  Self-regulatory depletion increases emotional reactivity in the amygdala. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[41]  Brent L. Hughes,et al.  Prefrontal-Subcortical Pathways Mediating Successful Emotion Regulation , 2008, Neuron.

[42]  B. Rasch,et al.  Suppressing Emotions Impairs Subsequent Stroop Performance and Reduces Prefrontal Brain Activation , 2013, PloS one.

[43]  K. Vohs,et al.  The Strength Model of Self-Control , 2007, Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine.

[44]  Matthew C. Keller,et al.  Increased sensitivity in neuroimaging analyses using robust regression , 2005, NeuroImage.

[45]  R. Davidson,et al.  The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  Patrick Dupont,et al.  Generalizable Representations of Pain, Cognitive Control, and Negative Emotion in Medial Frontal Cortex , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  T. Wager,et al.  Distinct Brain Systems Mediate the Effects of Nociceptive Input and Self-Regulation on Pain , 2015, PLoS biology.