Scrambling and Processing: Dependencies, Complexity, and Constraints

Two approaches characterize the current state of research in sentence processing. Both approaches aim to arrive at a well-grounded analysis of relevant constraints the construction is subject to. The first approach relies heavily on the hypothesis that a psycholinguistic investigation of a phenomenon can shed the light on some unresolved linguistic problem. The second approach advocated by cognitive science suggests that ideally we would study every construction in a language from both linguistic and psycholinguistic points of view. The first approach is firmly grounded in linguistics. Syntacticians are interested in the derivation of sentences and have ways of investigating how a sentence’s structural representation is defined by competence grammar. What distinguishes one syntactic theory from another is which the linguistic grounds it uses to argue for a particular structural representation. Often, theories compete with each other over long periods of time because linguistic methods do not strongly favor one theory over the other. One possible way to answer linguistic questions, suggested by Fodor (1993), is to investigate sentence structure using psycholinguistic methods that reveal how speakers and readers mentally represent such structure. The phenomenon of Scrambling is particularly fit to provide psycholinguistic evidence that may help to contrast different theories of grammar. These include generative grammar (Chomsky 1981; 1986; 1995) and the non-generative grammars such as Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) (Pollard and Sag, 1987; 1994), Lexical-Functional Grammar (Bresnan, 1998), and Categorial Grammar (Steedman, 2000). The Principles-and-Parameters version of the generative grammar assumes that arguments in a sentence derived by syntactic movement leave empty categories, that is, phonetically null placeholders in their original position. In contrast, the non-generative theories view empty categories, also known as traces, as theoretically undermotivated constructs and prefer accounts that do not require them. The second approach lies within the realm of contemporary cognitive science (Lepore and Pylyshyn, 1999). The syntactic study of word order variation cross-linguistically should contribute to a theory of the universal principles and constraints on language variation which govern Scrambling. It is also a necessary preliminary step in providing an account of how scrambled constructions are processed in sentence comprehension. Just as the theory of grammar has as its goal an account of Universal Grammar and parameters of language variation, the theory of sentence processing has as its goal the characterization of the universal parser, the human sentence processing mechanism. The theory of sentence processing has to address numerous questions. One question involves how movement constructions, usually referred to as filler-gap dependencies, are processed in general; another involves how scrambled constructions are processed in particular. Several theories of sentence processing argue for different accounts of Scrambling, but most of them start with a hypothesis that scrambled sentences are more complex than sentences in canonical word order. Complexity of a sentence can be broadly defined in structural terms (Fodor and Frazier, 1978; Frazier and Clifton, 1996), in terms of constraints it violates (MacDonald, Pearlmutter, and Seidenberg, 1994), in terms of computational resources (Gibson, 1998) or as working memory load (Just, Carpenter, and Hemphill, 1996). A considerable amount of experimental work has been done on Scrambling data from typologically diverse languages, using both the linguistic and psycholinguistic approaches. However, there is as yet no conclusive answer to the question of exactly how scrambled sentences are processed. Sentence processing experiments have been conducted in German, Japanese, Finnish, Serbo-Croatian, and Russian to establish the source of processing complexity of scrambled sentences in these languages, with the methodologies ranging from self-paced moving window technique to eye movement recording and cross-modal lexical priming. The studies will be compared in this chapter, and their implications for the linguistic and psycholinguistic theories of Scrambling will be discussed. The literature on this topic is growing rapidly, and in order to stay within the limits of a survey, this chapter will concentrate only on basic, clause-internal phrasal Scrambling of arguments. Other fascinating types of Scrambling, Long-Distance and Split Scrambling, will be left for another time. We will start with a brief introduction to sentence processing concepts and terms relevant to the subsequent discussion of processing of Scrambling.

[1]  J. Hyönä,et al.  Effects of Case Marking and Word Order on Sentence Parsing in Finnish: An Eye Fixation Analysis , 1997 .

[2]  Tracy Holloway King Configuring topic and focus in Russian , 1995 .

[3]  John A. Hawkins,et al.  A Performance Theory of Order and Constituency , 1995 .

[4]  G. Grewendorf,et al.  Scrambling and barriers , 1990 .

[5]  H. Clahsen,et al.  Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[6]  John Whitman,et al.  --; CHARLES CLIFTON; and JANET RANDALL. 1983. Filling gaps: Decision principles and structure in sentence comprehension. Cognition 13.187-222. , and JANET FODOR. 1978. The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition , 1997 .

[7]  J. Zwart The Minimalist Program , 1998, Journal of Linguistics.

[8]  E. T. Miyamoto Sources of difficulty in processing scrambling in Japanese , 2002 .

[9]  L Konieczny,et al.  Locality and Parsing Complexity , 2000, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[10]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  An Invitation to cognitive science , 1997 .

[11]  Thomas Wasow End-Weight from the Speaker's Perspective , 1997 .

[12]  Gisbert Fanselow,et al.  Scrambling as NP-Movement , 1990 .

[13]  Richard Shillcock,et al.  Cognitive models of speech processing : the Second Sperlonga Meeting , 1993 .

[14]  M. Gazzaniga The new cognitive neurosciences, 2nd ed. , 2000 .

[15]  Danijela. Stojanovic Parsing and acquisition: Evidence from Serbo-Croatian. , 1999 .

[16]  Edith Kaan,et al.  Processing subject-object ambiguities in Dutch , 1997 .

[17]  Tom Michael Mitchell,et al.  Mind Matters : A Tribute To Allen Newell , 1996 .

[18]  Paul Kaegbein,et al.  Methods in reading research , 1991 .

[19]  Janet Dean Fodor,et al.  The diagnosis and cure of garden paths , 1994 .

[20]  M. Coltheart Attention and Performance XII: The Psychology of Reading , 1987 .

[21]  G. Carpenter,et al.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 1999 .

[22]  G. Seth Psychology of Language , 1968, Nature.

[23]  C. Fiebach,et al.  WH‐Movement versus Scrambling: the Brain Makes a Difference , 2003 .

[24]  Reiko Mazuka,et al.  On Parameter Setting and Parsing: Predictions for Cross-Linguistic Differences in Adult and Child Processing , 1990 .

[25]  Daiko Takahashi,et al.  Scrambling and Last Resort , 1998, Linguistic Inquiry.

[26]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Speech, language, and communication , 1997 .

[27]  J. Kimball Seven principles of surface structure parsing in natural language , 1973 .

[28]  Hye-Won Choi,et al.  Optimizing structure in context: scrambling and information structure , 1996 .

[29]  Barbara Hemforth,et al.  Kognitives Parsing: Repräsentation und Verarbeitung sprachlichen Wissens , 1993, DISKI.

[30]  Janet Dean Fodor,et al.  Processing empty categories: A question of visibility. , 1993 .

[31]  Karin Humphreys,et al.  The psychology of language : from data to theory , 2001 .

[32]  G. Miller,et al.  Cognitive science. , 1981, Science.

[33]  Hiroaki Tada,et al.  A/A-bar partition in derivation , 1993 .

[34]  D Swinney,et al.  The role of structure in coreference assignment during sentence comprehension , 1989, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[35]  J. Fodor,et al.  The Psychology of Language , 1974 .

[36]  Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al.  The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution , 1994 .

[37]  Ivan A. Sag,et al.  Book Reviews: Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar and German in Head-driven Phrase-structure Grammar , 1996, CL.

[38]  Colin M. Brown,et al.  ERP effects of listening to speech compared to reading: the P600/SPS to syntactic violations in spoken sentences and rapid serial visual presentation , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Lectures on Government and Binding , 1981 .

[40]  Julie C. Sedivy,et al.  Resolving attachment ambiguities with multiple constraints , 1995, Cognition.

[41]  Olga T. Yokoyama,et al.  Discourse and word order , 1986 .

[42]  Markus Bader,et al.  Subject-Object Ambiguities in German Embedded Clauses: An Across-the-Board Comparison , 1999 .

[43]  Christoph Scheepers,et al.  Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution In German , 1998 .

[44]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  The new cognitive neurosciences , 2000 .

[45]  Bradley L. Pritchett Grammatical Competence and Parsing Performance , 1992 .

[46]  H. Clahsen,et al.  Antecedent Priming at Trace Positions: Evidence from German Scrambling , 1999 .

[47]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The architecture of working memory , 1997 .

[48]  Martin Kay,et al.  Syntactic Process , 1979, ACL.

[49]  Marica de Vincenzi,et al.  Syntactic parsing strategies in Italian , 1991 .

[50]  R. Mazuka,et al.  Japanese Sentence Processing , 1996 .

[51]  John Frederick Bailyn,et al.  A configurational approach to Russian "free" word order , 1995 .

[52]  C. Pollard,et al.  Center for the Study of Language and Information , 2022 .

[53]  Janet D. Fodor,et al.  The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model , 1978, Cognition.

[54]  Ivan A. Sag,et al.  Information-based syntax and semantics , 1987 .

[55]  Julie C. Sedivy,et al.  Subject Terms: Linguistics Language Eyes & eyesight Cognition & reasoning , 1995 .

[56]  Shigeru Miyagawa Against optinonal scrambling , 1997 .

[57]  M. Saito Some asymmetries in Japanese and their theoretical implications , 1985 .

[58]  Lyn Frazier,et al.  Language Processing and Language Acquisition , 1990 .

[59]  Hiroko Yamashita,et al.  The Effects of Word-Order and Case Marking Information on the Processing of Japanese , 1997 .

[60]  Hubert Haider,et al.  Scrambling — What’s the State of the Art? , 2000 .

[61]  J. Bresnan Lexical-Functional Syntax , 2000 .

[62]  Ernest Lepore,et al.  What is Cognitive Science , 1999 .

[63]  Shigeru Miyagawa Against Optional Scrambling , 2008 .

[64]  E. Gibson Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies , 1998, Cognition.

[65]  K. Rayner,et al.  Eye movements in reading: Psycholinguistic studies. , 1994 .

[66]  Patrizia Tabossi,et al.  Cross-Modal Semantic Priming , 1996 .

[67]  Wolfgang Sternefeld,et al.  Improper movement and unambiguous binding , 1993 .

[68]  Lyn Frazier,et al.  Sentence processing: A tutorial review. , 1987 .

[69]  M. de Vincenzi,et al.  Syntactic Parsing Strategies in Italian: The Minimal Chain Principle , 1991 .

[70]  Richard L. Lewis Interference in short-term memory: The magical number two (or three) in sentence processing , 1996, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[71]  Young-Suk Lee,et al.  Scrambling as case-driven obligatory movement , 1993 .

[72]  L. Frazier,et al.  Filler driven parsing: A study of gap filling in dutch , 1989 .

[73]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Word order and inflectional strategies in syntactic processing , 1988 .

[74]  Simin Karimi,et al.  Word order and scrambling , 2003 .

[75]  N. Kondrashova,et al.  The syntax of existential quantification , 1996 .

[76]  Anoop Mahajan,et al.  The A/A-bar distinction and movement theory , 1990 .

[77]  A. Inoue,et al.  Information-paced parsing of Japanese , 1995 .

[78]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Sentence processing without empty categories , 1991 .