Electrophysiological correlates of residual switch costs

Switching among cognitive tasks results in switch costs which are only partly reduced even after sufficient task preparation. These residual switch costs are frequently explained in terms of interference of simultaneously active task representations that delays selection of a correct response. Recent studies showed that the benefit of a task- and response-set repetition can also explain residual costs. We aimed to extend the findings by clarifying the mechanisms underlying task- and response-mode repetition benefit as well as costs arising by switch of one or both dimensions. To this end we used a combination of task-switching and go/no-go paradigm during an electrophysiological recording. Particularly, we focused on the frontocentral N2, which has been usually related to conflict, but also to response selection. The behavioral results replicate previous findings of lack of residual switch costs due to slower responses in task repetitions (TRs) following no-go relative to go trials. This indicates elimination of TR benefit when in a previous trial no response was selected and prepared. In other words, task sets clearly benefits from repetition of response mode whereas interference seems to occur whenever the task-set, the response mode or both were switched. Trial incongruity increased reaction times. The event-related potentials (ERPs) revealed a frontocentral N2 in all conditions which followed the same pattern as the reaction times (RTs), showing smaller amplitude and peaking earlier when both the task and response mode were repeated relative to the three switching conditions. Similar to the behavioral data, the N2 increased as a function of incongruity. Finally, both the N2 amplitude and latency correspond closely to the residual switch costs. This finding suggests that task-set or response mode switching intensify and delay response selection, relative to the repetition of both dimensions.

[1]  C. Carter,et al.  The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.

[2]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Response selection and response execution in task switching: evidence from a go-signal paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[3]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[4]  D. E. Astle,et al.  Dissociating neural indices of dynamic cognitive control in advance task-set preparation: An ERP study of task switching , 2006, Brain Research.

[5]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory task-switching processes. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[6]  W. B. Matthews Motivation, motor and sensory processes of the brain: electrical potentials, behaviour and clinical use. Proceedings of the 5th International Symposium Ulm-Reisensburg. May 14-18, 1979. , 1982, Progress in brain research.

[7]  H G Vaughan,et al.  Manipulation of event-related potential manifestations of information processing stages. , 1982, Science.

[8]  Paul D. Kieffaber,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of task switching and switch costs. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[9]  A. Kiesel,et al.  Task switching: on the origin of response congruency effects , 2007, Psychological research.

[10]  F. Karayanidis,et al.  Components of task-set reconfiguration: Differential effects of ‘switch-to’ and ‘switch-away’ cues , 2006, Brain Research.

[11]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[12]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Effects of response selection on the task repetition benefit in task switching , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[13]  R. Cunnington,et al.  ERP correlates of executive control during repeated language switching , 2001 .

[14]  W. Ritter,et al.  The scalp topography of potentials associated with missing visual or auditory stimuli. , 1976, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[15]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[16]  H. Heuer,et al.  Task Sets under Reconstruction: Effects of Partially Incorrect Precues , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[17]  Inhibition of wrong responses and conflict resolution: an electroencephalogram study , 2007, Neuroreport.

[18]  W. Ritter,et al.  The scalp topography of potentials in auditory and visual discrimination tasks. , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[19]  Iring Koch,et al.  Inhibition of response mode in task switching. , 2004, Experimental psychology.

[20]  B. Kopp,et al.  N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[21]  A C Nobre,et al.  Components of attentional set-switching. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[22]  Thomas Kleinsorge,et al.  Preparation for a forthcoming task is sufficient to produce subsequent shift costs , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  B. Renault,et al.  Correct and incorrect responses in a choice reaction time task and the endogenous components of the evoked potential. , 1980, Progress in brain research.

[24]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  D Friedman,et al.  A brain event related to the making of a sensory discrimination. , 1979, Science.

[26]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Impact of Cognitive Deficits on Conflict Monitoring , 2006, Psychological science.

[27]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[28]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[29]  J. Jonides,et al.  The mind's eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in internal attention shifting. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[30]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  Components of Switching Intentional Set , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[32]  H. Jasper Report of the committee on methods of clinical examination in electroencephalography , 1958 .

[33]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[34]  H G Vaughan,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of two stages of information processing in physical and semantic discrimination tasks. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[35]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  Short cue presentations encourage advance task preparation: a recipe to diminish the residual switch cost. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[37]  Dietrich Lehmann,et al.  Human Evoked Potentials , 1979 .

[38]  Thomas Goschke,et al.  Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective , 2003 .

[39]  R. Ulrich,et al.  No evidence for a late locus of task switch effects , 2009, Brain Research.

[40]  P Finkenzeller,et al.  Evoked responses as a function of external and stored information. , 1968, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[41]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory and poststimulus components of task switching. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[42]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Can the task-cuing paradigm measure an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process? , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[44]  C. Rorden,et al.  Cognitive Control Mechanisms Revealed by ERP and fMRI: Evidence from Repeated Task-Switching , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  E. Wascher,et al.  Shifting attention between global features and small details: an event-related potential study , 1997, Biological Psychology.

[46]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Task switching: a high-density electrical mapping study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[47]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[48]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  ERP—Correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm , 2008, Brain Research.

[49]  I. Koch,et al.  The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  Hong Li,et al.  The timing of cognitive control in partially incongruent categorization , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[51]  B. Kopp,et al.  N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[52]  B. Renault,et al.  A Trial by Trial Study of the Visual Omission Response in Reaction Time Situations , 1979 .

[53]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .