The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in the Control of Imitation
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Saxe,et al. Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[2] Iroise Dumontheil,et al. Taking perspective into account in a communicative task , 2010, NeuroImage.
[3] J. R. Simon,et al. Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[4] J. Decety,et al. The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[5] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] Geoffrey Bird,et al. Enhancing Social Ability by Stimulating Right Temporoparietal Junction Tdcs of Right Tpj , 2022 .
[7] Dana Samson,et al. Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[8] R. Rafal,et al. Visual Extinction and Stimulus Repetition , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[9] T. Chartrand,et al. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[10] M. Corbetta,et al. False Belief vs. False Photographs: A Test of Theory of Mind or Working Memory? , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[11] M. Brass,et al. The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: a functional double dissociation , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[12] Paul E. Downing,et al. Facilitation and interference in spatial and body reference frames , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al. Development of the social brain in adolescence , 2012, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.
[14] R. Saxe,et al. What gets the attention of the temporo-parietal junction? An fMRI investigation of attention and theory of mind , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[15] G. Bird,et al. Social attitudes differentially modulate imitation in adolescents and adults , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] Matthew R Longo,et al. Imitative response tendencies following observation of intransitive actions. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] Marcel Brass,et al. Resisting motor mimicry: Control of imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with frontal and temporo-parietal lesions , 2010, Social neuroscience.
[18] Sara Torriero,et al. Prefrontal and Temporo-Parietal Involvement in Taking Others’ Perspective: TMS Evidence , 2008, Behavioural neurology.
[19] S. Blakemore,et al. Social cognitive neuroscience: where are we heading? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[20] Robin I. M. Dunbar. The social brain meets neuroimaging , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] Emery N. Brown,et al. Distinct Regions of Right Temporo-Parietal Junction Are Selective for Theory of Mind and Exogenous Attention , 2009, PloS one.
[22] R. Adolphs,et al. The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.
[23] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Research Note Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 1992 .
[24] C. Heyes,et al. Hand to mouth: automatic imitation across effector systems. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] Caroline Catmur,et al. Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] F. Van Overwalle. Social cognition and the brain: A meta‐analysis , 2009, Human brain mapping.
[27] J. Sommerville,et al. Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[28] T. Insel,et al. How the brain processes social information: searching for the social brain. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[29] Beate Sodian,et al. Common and distinct neural networks for false-belief reasoning and inhibitory control , 2011, NeuroImage.
[30] Jason P. Mitchell. Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[31] Beth Stevens,et al. TGF-β Signaling Regulates Neuronal C1q Expression and Developmental Synaptic Refinement , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.
[32] J. Perner,et al. The Curious Incident of the Photo that was Accused of Being False: Issues of Domain Specificity in Development, Autism, and Brain Imaging , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[33] R. Rafal,et al. A systematic study of visual extinction. Between- and within-field deficits of attention in hemispatial neglect. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[34] T. Singer,et al. The Social Neuroscience of Empathy , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[35] M. Brass,et al. Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[36] P. C. Murphy,et al. Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.
[37] J. Driver,et al. Mechanisms and anatomy of unilateral extinction after brain injury , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[38] W. Prinz,et al. Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.
[39] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[40] Marcel Brass,et al. Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution , 2009, Human brain mapping.
[41] C. Heyes,et al. Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.