The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in the Control of Imitation

Controlling neural representations of the self and other people is fundamental to social cognition. Brain imaging studies have implicated the right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ) in this ability, but causal evidence for its role is lacking. A debate is also ongoing regarding whether the control of, or switching between, self and other representations is a specialized or domain-general process: the rTPJ's well-established role in reorienting attention supports a domain-general process, but a role specific to social cognition has also been proposed. Neuronavigated repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation was used to target the rTPJ (and a control mid-occipital site) during a task requiring participants to switch between representations of others' actions on both a social and a nonsocial level, by manipulating imitative and spatial compatibility simultaneously and independently. Both imitative and spatial compatibility effects were apparent on response times; however, the effect of imitative compatibility was significantly stronger, indicating less control of imitation, during stimulation of the rTPJ relative to the control site. This suggests that the rTPJ is involved in switching between self and other representations, and further, that this process may not be entirely domain general.

[1]  R. Saxe,et al.  Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Taking perspective into account in a communicative task , 2010, NeuroImage.

[3]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[5]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Enhancing Social Ability by Stimulating Right Temporoparietal Junction Tdcs of Right Tpj , 2022 .

[7]  Dana Samson,et al.  Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  R. Rafal,et al.  Visual Extinction and Stimulus Repetition , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  M. Corbetta,et al.  False Belief vs. False Photographs: A Test of Theory of Mind or Working Memory? , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[11]  M. Brass,et al.  The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: a functional double dissociation , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Paul E. Downing,et al.  Facilitation and interference in spatial and body reference frames , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Development of the social brain in adolescence , 2012, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[14]  R. Saxe,et al.  What gets the attention of the temporo-parietal junction? An fMRI investigation of attention and theory of mind , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  G. Bird,et al.  Social attitudes differentially modulate imitation in adolescents and adults , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Matthew R Longo,et al.  Imitative response tendencies following observation of intransitive actions. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Resisting motor mimicry: Control of imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with frontal and temporo-parietal lesions , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[18]  Sara Torriero,et al.  Prefrontal and Temporo-Parietal Involvement in Taking Others’ Perspective: TMS Evidence , 2008, Behavioural neurology.

[19]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: where are we heading? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  Robin I. M. Dunbar The social brain meets neuroimaging , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Emery N. Brown,et al.  Distinct Regions of Right Temporo-Parietal Junction Are Selective for Theory of Mind and Exogenous Attention , 2009, PloS one.

[22]  R. Adolphs,et al.  The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[23]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Research Note Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 1992 .

[24]  C. Heyes,et al.  Hand to mouth: automatic imitation across effector systems. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  F. Van Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: A meta‐analysis , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[27]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  T. Insel,et al.  How the brain processes social information: searching for the social brain. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[29]  Beate Sodian,et al.  Common and distinct neural networks for false-belief reasoning and inhibitory control , 2011, NeuroImage.

[30]  Jason P. Mitchell Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  Beth Stevens,et al.  TGF-β Signaling Regulates Neuronal C1q Expression and Developmental Synaptic Refinement , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  J. Perner,et al.  The Curious Incident of the Photo that was Accused of Being False: Issues of Domain Specificity in Development, Autism, and Brain Imaging , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  R. Rafal,et al.  A systematic study of visual extinction. Between- and within-field deficits of attention in hemispatial neglect. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  T. Singer,et al.  The Social Neuroscience of Empathy , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[35]  M. Brass,et al.  Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[37]  J. Driver,et al.  Mechanisms and anatomy of unilateral extinction after brain injury , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[39]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[41]  C. Heyes,et al.  Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.