Analytic Thinking Promotes Religious Disbelief

Overthinking Religion? Many theories of human cognition make a distinction between System I, which tends to be rapid and to rely on heuristics or rules of thumb, and System II, which tends to be more deliberative and analytic. This dual-process framework, within which both processes may operate simultaneously and competitively, has been used to explain a variety of situational influences upon decision-making. Gervais and Norenzayan (p. 493) studied the application of a dual-process framework to religious disbelief and found that triggering analytic thinking processes through a variety of experimental manipulations resulted in a tendency for subjects to report lower levels of religious belief. A dual-process theory posits a competition between analytical thought and religious belief. Scientific interest in the cognitive underpinnings of religious belief has grown in recent years. However, to date, little experimental research has focused on the cognitive processes that may promote religious disbelief. The present studies apply a dual-process model of cognitive processing to this problem, testing the hypothesis that analytic processing promotes religious disbelief. Individual differences in the tendency to analytically override initially flawed intuitions in reasoning were associated with increased religious disbelief. Four additional experiments provided evidence of causation, as subtle manipulations known to trigger analytic processing also encouraged religious disbelief. Combined, these studies indicate that analytic processing is one factor (presumably among several) that promotes religious disbelief. Although these findings do not speak directly to conversations about the inherent rationality, value, or truth of religious beliefs, they illuminate one cognitive factor that may influence such discussions.

[1]  E. Uhlmann,et al.  Implicit Puritanism in American moral cognition , 2011 .

[2]  S. Levinson,et al.  WEIRD languages have misled us, too , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[3]  A. Norenzayan,et al.  The weirdest people in the world? , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[4]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Believers' estimates of God's beliefs are more egocentric than estimates of other people's beliefs , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  D. Kelemen,et al.  The Human Function Compunction: Teleological explanation in adults , 2009, Cognition.

[6]  Dimitrios Kapogiannis,et al.  Cognitive and neural foundations of religious belief , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  FLUENCY AND THE DETECTION OF MISLEADING QUESTIONS : LOW PROCESSING FLUENCY ATTENUATES THE MOSES ILLUSION , 2008 .

[8]  Pascal Boyer,et al.  Being human: Religion: Bound to believe? , 2008, Nature.

[9]  A. Shariff,et al.  The Origin and Evolution of Religious Prosociality , 2008, Science.

[10]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Overcoming intuition: metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  J. Bering The folk psychology of souls , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[12]  P. Harris,et al.  Trust in testimony: how children learn about science and religion. , 2006, Child development.

[13]  Craig K Enders,et al.  The varieties of religious development in adulthood: a longitudinal investigation of religion and rational choice. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  F. Strack,et al.  Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[15]  D. Kelemen Are Children “Intuitive Theists”? , 2004, Psychological science.

[16]  Jonathan Evans In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[18]  J. Barrett Exploring the natural foundations of religion , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  R. Boyd,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. , 1998 .

[20]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Thinking too much: introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  R. Larsen,et al.  The Satisfaction with Life Scale , 1985, Journal of personality assessment.

[22]  P. Pollard,et al.  On the conflict between logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[23]  R. Revlin,et al.  The belief-bias effect in formal reasoning: The influence of knowledge on logic , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[24]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Category accessibility and social perception: Some implications for the study of person memory and interpersonal judgments , 1980 .

[25]  D. R. Hoge A validated intrinsic religious motivation scale. , 1972 .

[26]  Søren Kierkegaard,et al.  Fear and Trembling , 1843 .

[27]  David G. Rand,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Divine Intuition: Cognitive Style Influences Belief in God , 2011 .

[28]  Paul Bloom,et al.  Religion is natural. , 2007, Developmental science.

[29]  P. Norris,et al.  Sacred and Secular: Religion and Politics Worldwide , 2004 .

[30]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[31]  S. Frederick,et al.  Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .