Children's Awareness of Authority to Change Rules in Various Social Contexts

To investigate children’s awareness of authority to change rules, we showed children (ages 4-7) videos of one child playing a game alone or three children playing a game together. In the group video, the game rule was initiated either: by one of the children, by three children collaboratively or by an adult. They then were asked whether the characters in the videos could change the rules. Children believed that the character could change the rule when playing alone. Their responses to the group video depended on how the rule was initiated. They attributed authority to change rules only to the child who initiated the rule, unless the rule was created collaboratively. We also asked children whether they could change norms (school/moral/artifact norms) in daily life; and found moral/artifact distinction in children’s endorsement of norm changing. These results suggest that children recognize flexibility in changing rules even in preschool years.

[1]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Children's norm enforcement in their interactions with peers. , 2014, Child development.

[2]  Marjorie Taylor,et al.  Imaginary Companions and the Children Who Create Them , 1999 .

[3]  Charles W. Kalish,et al.  Children's Reasoning about Norms and Traits as Motives for Behavior. , 2004 .

[4]  J. Piaget The Moral Judgment of the Child , 1932 .

[5]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Young children's understanding of the context-relativity of normative rules in conventional games. , 2009, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[6]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  The sources of normativity: young children's awareness of the normative structure of games. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[7]  M. Schoen The Moral Judgment of the Child. , 1933 .

[8]  Judith G. Smetana Preschool Children's Conceptions of Moral and Social Rules. , 1981 .

[9]  L. Markson,et al.  Children's Reliance on Creator's Intent in Extending Names for Artifacts , 2003, Psychological science.

[10]  T. Kushnir,et al.  Pedagogical cues encourage toddlers' transmission of recently demonstrated functions to unfamiliar adults. , 2015, Developmental science.

[11]  Krista Casler,et al.  Young children's rapid learning about artifacts. , 2005, Developmental science.

[12]  D. Kelemen Are Children “Intuitive Theists”? , 2004, Psychological science.

[13]  R. Blair The development of morality , 1992 .

[14]  Marta Laupa Children's reasoning about authority in home and school contexts , 1995 .

[15]  C. Lawson,et al.  Development of social category representations: early appreciation of roles and deontic relations. , 2008, Child development.

[16]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Normativity and context in young children's pretend play , 2009 .

[17]  Qi Wang,et al.  A Comparison of American and Nepalese Children's Concepts of Freedom of Choice and Social Constraint , 2013, Cogn. Sci..

[18]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Collaborative partner or social tool? New evidence for young children's understanding of joint intentions in collaborative activities. , 2012, Developmental science.

[19]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Young children's creation and transmission of social norms , 2014 .

[20]  Penny Munn,et al.  Development of justification in disputes with mother and sibling. , 1987 .

[21]  Ori Friedman,et al.  Young children give priority to ownership when judging who should use an object. , 2014, Child development.

[22]  Tamar Kushnir,et al.  The Self as a Moral Agent: Preschoolers Behave Morally but Believe in the Freedom to Do Otherwise , 2014 .

[23]  Larry Nucci,et al.  Children's social interactions in the context of moral and conventional transgressions. , 1982 .

[24]  Hannes Rakoczy,et al.  Taking fiction seriously: young children understand the normative structure of joint pretence games. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[25]  Penny Munn,et al.  Becoming A Family Member: Family Conflict and the Development of Social Understanding in the Second Year. , 1985 .