Number generation bias after action observation

Recent studies have demonstrated that conceptual and abstract knowledge could rely on and could be influenced by sensory-motor processing of usual goal-directed actions. In line with this, interactions have been reported between number magnitude and finger grip with, for example, small-magnitude numbers priming grip closure and large-magnitude numbers priming grip aperture. Here, we assessed whether observing a closing or opening grip was able to influence the magnitude of the numbers produced in a random number generation task, and we tested whether this effect was specific to biological hand actions by using non-biological fake hands with the same closure or aperture amplitude. The participants were asked to produce as randomly as possible numbers between 1 and 10 after they observed a change in posture (i.e. grip closing or grip opening) or in colour (i.e. red or blue hand). The results revealed that the participants produced more often small numbers than large ones after observing a grip closing, whereas they produced equally often small and large numbers after observing a grip opening or colour changes. Importantly, this effect was only present for the biological hands but not for the non-biological fake hands. This finding demonstrates that observing a biological grip closing conveys small-magnitude information, which, in turn, influences the mental selection of a numerical response. We discuss our results in the light of the internal random generator process proposed in the domain of numerical cognition and argue that number semantics is stored with a code governed by sensory-motor mechanisms.

[1]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  T. Loetscher,et al.  Exploring number space by random digit generation , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Jody C. Culham,et al.  fMRI Activation during Observation of Others' Reach Errors , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[5]  J. Decety,et al.  Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta‐analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[6]  V. Walsh,et al.  The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  T. Loetscher,et al.  Eye position predicts what number you have in mind , 2010, Current Biology.

[8]  Michael Andres,et al.  Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction , 2004, Neuroreport.

[9]  F. Mast,et al.  Moving along the mental number line: Interactions between whole-body motion and numerical cognition. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Numeric comparison in a visually-guided manual reaching task , 2008, Cognition.

[11]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Getting a grip on numbers: numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  T. Loetscher,et al.  Head turns bias the brain's internal random generator , 2008, Current Biology.

[13]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  J. A. Adams Use of the model's knowledge of results to increase the observer's performance , 1986 .

[15]  R. J. Seitz,et al.  A fronto‐parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI‐study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[16]  Michael Andres,et al.  Number magnitude potentiates action judgements , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  J. Hoffmann,et al.  Anticipated Action Effects Affect the Selection, Initiation, and Execution of Actions , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[18]  Mauro PESENTI,et al.  Neuroanatomical substrates of Arabic number processing, numerical comparison and simple addition: A PET study. , 1998, NeuroImage.

[19]  Arnaud Badets,et al.  Creating number semantics through finger movement perception , 2010, Cognition.

[20]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  A. Idrissi,et al.  On the Mental Representation of , 2008 .

[22]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[23]  Arnaud Badets,et al.  Finger-number interaction: an ideomotor account. , 2011, Experimental psychology.

[24]  Mauro Pesenti,et al.  Age-Related Differences in Random Generation , 1998, Brain and Cognition.