The Roles of Timing and Task Order during Task Switching

The neural bases of the different processes involved in task switching remain poorly identified. Whether distinct brain regions are involved according to the overall structure of the task sequence and the predictability of task timing during task switching is unknown. To address this question, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging and a 2 x 2 factorial design varying timing (fixed/random) and task order (predictable/unpredictable). We hypothesized that predictable task order should activate brain regions involved in long-term memory retrieval because retrieving which task has to be performed constitutes the essential part of what subjects can do to prepare before stimulus presentation. When examining the "pure" main effects of task order/timing predictability/ unpredictability, we found that anticipating task order activated the right hippocampus, the anterior medial prefrontal cortex, and the posterior cingulate cortex, while anticipating task onset timing activated the left middle and superior frontal gyrus. Furthermore, task order unpredictability activated the intraparietal cortex bilaterally while random relative to fixed timing activated the right cerebellum. Interactions between task order and timing were found in a network, which included the left frontopolar cortex and the lateral prefrontal cortex bilaterally. Specifically, the left frontopolar cortex was more activated when both timing and task order were predictable, while the lateral prefrontal cortices were more activated when both task order and timing were unpredictable. These results indicate a hierarchic organization of the prefrontal cortex along a posterioanterior axis as the task becomes more endogenously guided. Finally, we found no evidence for specific brain regions involved in task switching because a bilateral prefronto-parietal network, which was activated in task switching relative to performing each task separately, was no longer activated relative to a control condition, which required subjects to maintain two tasks in memory without switching between them.

[1]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  P. J. Foley The foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1959, Canadian journal of psychology.

[4]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  D Y von Cramon,et al.  Executive control functions in task switching: evidence from brain injured patients. , 1999, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[6]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  A neural system for human visual working memory. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Prediction and preparation, fundamental functions of the cerebellum. , 1997, Learning & memory.

[8]  E. Maguire,et al.  Differential modulation of a common memory retrieval network revealed by positron emission tomography , 1999, Hippocampus.

[9]  R. Ivry The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[10]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[11]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval , 1999, Nature.

[13]  I. Biederman,et al.  Mental set and mental shift revisited , 1976 .

[14]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Contribution of working memory to transient activation in human inferior prefrontal cortex during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  D. Tucker,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex in Action Monitoring , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[17]  A Baddeley,et al.  Random Generation and the Executive Control of Working Memory , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[18]  J. Price,et al.  Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[19]  J. Assad,et al.  Dynamic coding of behaviourally relevant stimuli in parietal cortex , 2002, Nature.

[20]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Dissociating the role of the medial and lateral anterior prefrontal cortex in human planning. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Modulation of task-related neural activity in task-switching: an fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[22]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .

[23]  D. Schacter,et al.  Medial temporal lobe activations in fMRI and PET studies of episodic encoding and retrieval , 1999, Hippocampus.

[24]  P M Grasby,et al.  Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory. An in vivo study in humans. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  J D Gabrieli,et al.  A resource model of the neural basis of executive working memory. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Visual attention: Insights from brain imaging , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  Strategies and automaticity. I: Basic findings and conceptual framework , 1994 .

[28]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[29]  P. Goldman-Rakic The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  C. Frith,et al.  Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[34]  A. Owen The Functional Organization of Working Memory Processes Within Human Lateral Frontal Cortex: The Contribution of Functional Neuroimaging , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[35]  R. A. Carlson,et al.  Effects of repetition and foreknowledge in task-set reconfiguration. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Comparing Functional (PET) Images: The Assessment of Significant Change , 1991, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[37]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociating executive mechanisms of task control following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson's disease. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  M. Piercy,et al.  A case of persisting impairment of recent memory following right temporal lobectomy , 1964 .

[39]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[41]  D Gopher,et al.  Switching tasks and attention policies. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[42]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[43]  S. Zeki,et al.  Three cortical stages of colour processing in the human brain. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  H. Barbas,et al.  Topographically specific hippocampal projections target functionally distinct prefrontal areas in the rhesus monkey , 1995, Hippocampus.

[45]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  S Monsell,et al.  Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? , 2000, Psychological research.

[47]  G. Radvansky,et al.  Studies of directed forgetting in older adults. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[48]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[49]  D. Pandya,et al.  Fiber system linking the mid‐dorsolateral frontal cortex with the retrosplenial/presubicular region in the rhesus monkey , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[50]  C. Price,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomy of the Semantic System: Divisible by What? , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  K. Arbuthnott,et al.  Executive control in set switching: residual switch cost and task-set inhibition. , 2000, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[52]  L. Jäncke,et al.  Tapping movements according to regular and irregular visual timing signals investigated with fMRI , 2000, Neuroreport.

[53]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Transient activation of inferior prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Andrew Simmons,et al.  Prefrontal involvement in temporal bridging and timing movement , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[56]  J. A. Frost,et al.  Conceptual Processing during the Conscious Resting State: A Functional MRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  Endel Tulving,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval mode. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[58]  A. Dove,et al.  Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[59]  A. Dale,et al.  Late Onset of Anterior Prefrontal Activity during True and False Recognition: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 1997, NeuroImage.

[60]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[61]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition , 1999, Nature.

[62]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex , 2000, Psychobiology.

[63]  J. Lisman Relating Hippocampal Circuitry to Function Recall of Memory Sequences by Reciprocal Dentate–CA3 Interactions , 1999, Neuron.

[64]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of executive processes involved in dual-task performance. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[65]  E. Tulving,et al.  Task-related and item-related brain processes of memory retrieval. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[66]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.