Precision and power in the analysis of social structure using associations

I develop guidelines for assessing the precision and power of statistical techniques that are frequently used to study nonhuman social systems using observed dyadic associations. Association indexes estimate the proportion of time that two individuals are associated. Binomial approximation and nonparametric bootstrap methods produce similar estimates of the precision of association indexes. For a mid-range (0.4–0.9) association index to have a standard error of less than 0.1 requires about 15 observations of the pair associated, and for it to be less than 0.05, this rises to 50 observations. The coefficient of variation among dyads of the proportion of time that pairs of individuals are actually associated describes social differentiation (S), and this may be estimated from association data using maximum likelihood. With a poorly differentiated population (S ∼ 0.2), a data set needs about five observed associations per dyad to achieve a correlation between true and estimated association indexes of r = ∼0.4. It requires about 10 times as much data to achieve a representation with r = ∼0.8. Permutation tests usually reject the null hypothesis that individuals have no preferred associates when S2 × H > 5, where H is the mean number of observed associations per individual. Thus most situations require substantial numbers of observations of associations to give useful portrayals of social systems, and sparse association data inform only when social differentiation is high.

[1]  Maud Poisbleau,et al.  Assessing the reliability of dominance scores for assigning individual ranks in a hierarchy , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[2]  Hal Whitehead,et al.  Selection of Models of Lagged Identification Rates and Lagged Association Rates Using AIC and QAIC , 2007, Commun. Stat. Simul. Comput..

[3]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Testing association patterns of social animals , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  Han de Vries,et al.  An improved test of linearity in dominance hierarchies containing unknown or tied relationships , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  Steven J. Schwager,et al.  A comparison of association indices , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  Hilde Vervaecke,et al.  Measuring and testing the steepness of dominance hierarchies , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  H. Whitehead Analysing animal social structure , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  Allan Jones,et al.  Experimental design for the Life Sciences (2nd Edition) , 2006 .

[9]  Truman P. Young,et al.  Measuring association between individuals or groups in behavioural studies , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  S. Hooker,et al.  Social organization in northern bottlenose whales, Hyperoodon ampullatus: not driven by deep-water foraging? , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[11]  L. Bejder,et al.  A method for testing association patterns of social animals , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[12]  Hal Whitehead,et al.  Techniques for Analyzing Vertebrate Social Structure Using Identified Individuals: Review and Recommendations , 1999 .

[13]  L. C. Cole,et al.  The Measurement of Interspecific Associaton , 1949 .

[14]  Peter K. McGregor,et al.  Analyzing Animal Societies: Quantitative Methods for Vertebrate Social Analysis , 2009 .

[15]  E. B. Wilson Probable Inference, the Law of Succession, and Statistical Inference , 1927 .

[16]  Lars Bejder,et al.  Testing association patterns: issues arising and extensions , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  R. A. Hinde,et al.  Interactions, Relationships and Social Structure , 1976 .

[18]  B. Efron,et al.  A Leisurely Look at the Bootstrap, the Jackknife, and , 1983 .

[19]  B. Morgan,et al.  VISUALIZING INTERACTION AND SEQUENTIAL DATA IN ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR: THEORY AND APPLICATION OF CLUSTER-ANALYSIS METHODS , 1976 .