Prominence of Direct Entorhinal–CA1 Pathway Activation in Sensorimotor and Cognitive Tasks Revealed by 2-DG Functional Mapping in Nonhuman Primate

The trisynaptic pathway from entorhinal cortex to the hippocampus has long been regarded as the major route of information transfer underlying memory consolidation. Most physiological studies of this pathway involve recording from hippocampal slices. We have used both single- and double-label 2-deoxyglucose autoradiographic methods to image the pattern of activation in the hippocampal formation of 14 rhesus monkeys performing cognitive tasks, varying in content (spatial or nonspatial), process (working memory or associative memory), and mode of response (oculomotor or manual). These studies revealed a highly differentiated pattern of metabolic activation throughout the rostrocaudal extent of the hippocampal formation that was common to all behavioral conditions examined. This pattern consisted of intense activation of the stratum lacunosum-moleculare of CA1 and the subiculum, contrasting with barely detectable activity in CA3 and modest activation in the dentate gyrus, which did not include its molecular layer. These findings indicate a remarkable invariance in hippocampal activation under conditions of varied content, varied process, and varied mode of response and an heretofore-unappreciated preferential engagement of the direct rather than the trisynaptic pathway during performance of a wide range of behavioral tasks.

[1]  M Meunier,et al.  Effects of rhinal cortex lesions combined with hippocampectomy on visual recognition memory in rhesus monkeys. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  D. Amaral,et al.  Organization of CA1 projections to the subiculum: A PHA‐L analysis in the rat , 1991, Hippocampus.

[3]  M. Mishkin,et al.  A selective mnemonic role for the hippocampus in monkeys: memory for the location of objects , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  D J Brooks,et al.  Temporally‐specific retrograde amnesia in two cases of discrete bilateral hippocampal pathology , 1999, Hippocampus.

[5]  E T Rolls,et al.  Information about spatial view in an ensemble of primate hippocampal cells. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  J. O’Keefe Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[7]  R. Nicoll,et al.  Long-term potentiation--a decade of progress? , 1999, Science.

[8]  E. Tulving,et al.  Memory beyond the hippocampus , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[9]  D. Amaral,et al.  The entorhinal cortex of the monkey: II. Cortical afferents , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[10]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Object Recognition and Location Memory in Monkeys with Excitotoxic Lesions of the Amygdala and Hippocampus , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  D. Amaral,et al.  The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: A review of anatomical data , 1989, Neuroscience.

[12]  P S Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Activation of the hippocampus and dentate gyrus by working-memory: a 2- deoxyglucose study of behaving rhesus monkeys , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  D. Amaral,et al.  Lesions of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices in the monkey produce long-lasting memory impairment in the visual and tactual modalities , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  E. Murray,et al.  Monkeys with rhinal cortex damage or neurotoxic hippocampal lesions are impaired on spatial scene learning and object reversals. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dual pathways connecting the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with the hippocampal formation and parahippocampal cortex in the rhesus monkey , 1984, Neuroscience.

[16]  W. Scoville,et al.  Loss of Recent Memory After Bilateral Hippocampal Lesions , 2000 .

[17]  M Segal,et al.  Behavior of units in hippocampal circuit of the rat during learning. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  P Alvarez,et al.  Damage limited to the hippocampal region produces long-lasting memory impairment in monkeys , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  D. Amaral,et al.  Topographical organization of the entorhinal projection to the dentate gyrus of the monkey , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  D. C. Wright,et al.  The effect of unilateral and bilateral removal of the entorhinal cortex on the glucose utilization in various hippocampal regions in the rat , 1988, Neuroscience Letters.

[21]  B. Leonard,et al.  Transient memory impairment in monkeys with bilateral lesions of the entorhinal cortex , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  L R Squire,et al.  Retrograde Amnesia for Facts and Events: Findings from Four New Cases , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  M. Yeckel,et al.  Monosynaptic excitation of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells by afferents from the entorhinal cortex , 1995, Hippocampus.

[24]  H. Barbas,et al.  Topographically specific hippocampal projections target functionally distinct prefrontal areas in the rhesus monkey , 1995, Hippocampus.

[25]  T. Bliss,et al.  Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  L R Squire,et al.  Lesions of the hippocampal formation but not lesions of the fornix or the mammillary nuclei produce long-lasting memory impairment in monkeys , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  M Mishkin,et al.  Neural substrates of visual stimulus-stimulus association in rhesus monkeys , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[28]  E. Rolls,et al.  Activity of hippocampal formation neurons in the monkey related to a conditional spatial response task. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Severe tactual as well as visual memory deficits follow combined removal of the amygdala and hippocampus in monkeys , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  D. Amaral,et al.  Three Cases of Enduring Memory Impairment after Bilateral Damage Limited to the Hippocampal Formation , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Effects on visual recognition of combined and separate ablations of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortex in rhesus monkeys , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  J. Stevens,et al.  Bilateral lesions of CA1 and CA2 fields of the hippocampus are sufficient to cause a severe amnesic syndrome in humans. , 1995, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[33]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  C. B. Cave,et al.  Equivalent impairment of spatial and nonspatial memory following damage to the human hippocampus , 1991, Hippocampus.

[35]  E. Rolls,et al.  Spatial View Cells in the Primate Hippocampus , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  W. H. Mcmenemey,et al.  THE STRUCTURE OF AMMON'S HORN , 1969 .

[37]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Hippocampectomized monkeys can remember one place but not two , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  L. Sokoloff,et al.  Local cerebral glucose utilization in the normal conscious macaque monkey , 1978, Annals of neurology.

[39]  O. Steward,et al.  Topographic organization of the projections from the entorhinal area to the hippocampal formation of the rat , 1976, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[40]  I. Soltesz Brief history of cortico‐hippocampal time with a special reference to the direct entorhinal input to CA1 , 1995, Hippocampus.

[41]  M. Deschenes,et al.  Low- and high-frequency membrane potential oscillations during theta activity in CA1 and CA3 pyramidal neurons of the rat hippocampus under ketamine-xylazine anesthesia. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  J. O’Keefe Do hippocampal pyramidal cells signal non‐spatial as well as spatial information? , 1999, Hippocampus.

[43]  Hisao Nishijo,et al.  The relationship between monkey hippocampus place-related neural activity and action in space , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[44]  L R Squire,et al.  Impaired recognition memory in patients with lesions limited to the hippocampal formation. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.

[45]  M. Yeckel,et al.  Feedforward excitation of the hippocampus by afferents from the entorhinal cortex: redefinition of the role of the trisynaptic pathway. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  L. Sokoloff Sites and mechanisms of function-related changes in energy metabolism in the nervous system. , 1993, Developmental neuroscience.

[47]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Matching patterns of activity in primate prefrontal area 8a and parietal area 7ip neurons during a spatial working memory task. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  M. Reivich,et al.  THE [14C]DEOXYGLUCOSE METHOD FOR THE MEASUREMENT OF LOCAL CEREBRAL GLUCOSE UTILIZATION: THEORY, PROCEDURE, AND NORMAL VALUES IN THE CONSCIOUS AND ANESTHETIZED ALBINO RAT 1 , 1977, Journal of neurochemistry.

[49]  L. Squire,et al.  Lesions of the amygdala that spare adjacent cortical regions do not impair memory or exacerbate the impairment following lesions of the hippocampal formation , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[50]  M. Mishkin Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not by separate removal of amygdala and hippocampus , 1978, Nature.

[51]  D. Amaral,et al.  Lesions of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex that spare the amygdala and hippocampal formation produce severe memory impairment , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[52]  F. H. Lopes da Silva,et al.  Anatomic organization and physiology of the limbic cortex. , 1990, Physiological reviews.

[53]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[54]  G Buzsáki,et al.  Possible physiological role of the perforant path‐CA1 projection , 1995, Hippocampus.

[55]  M. Mishkin A memory system in the monkey. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[56]  Robert F. Sproull,et al.  Principles in interactive computer graphics , 1973 .

[57]  CJ Bruce,et al.  Resolution of metabolic columns by a double-label 2-DG technique: interdigitation and coincidence in visual cortical areas of the same monkey , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[58]  I. Soltesz,et al.  The direct perforant path input to CA1: Excitatory or inhibitory? , 1995, Hippocampus.

[59]  Menno P. Witter,et al.  Entorhinal projections to the hippocampal CA1 region in the rat: An underestimated pathway , 1988, Neuroscience Letters.

[60]  M. Witter Organization of the entorhinal—hippocampal system: A review of current anatomical data , 1993, Hippocampus.

[61]  L. Squire,et al.  Memory impairment in monkeys following lesions limited to the hippocampus. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[62]  L. Squire,et al.  Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[63]  D. Amaral,et al.  Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: Cortical afferents , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[64]  A R McIntosh,et al.  Positron emission tomography correlations in and beyond medial temporal lobes , 1999, Hippocampus.

[65]  E. Murray,et al.  Monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) with rhinal cortex ablations succeed in object discrimination learning despite 24-hr intertrial intervals and fail at matching to sample despite double sample presentations. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[66]  John O'Keefe,et al.  Hippocampus, theta, and spatial memory , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[67]  D. Olton,et al.  Spatial memory and hippocampal function , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[68]  L. Sokoloff Function-related changes in energy metabolism in the nervous system: localization and mechanisms. , 1993, The Keio journal of medicine.

[69]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Visual recognition in monkeys following rhinal cortical ablations combined with either amygdalectomy or hippocampectomy , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[70]  E. Rolls Spatial view cells and the representation of place in the primate hippocampus , 1999, Hippocampus.

[71]  F. F. Weight,et al.  Perforant pathway activation of hippocampal CA1 stratum pyramidale neurons: Electrophysiological evidence for a direct pathway , 1982, Brain Research.

[72]  Roland S. G. Jones Entorhinal-hippocampal connections: a speculative view of their function , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[73]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.