Universality Versus Language-Specificity in Listening to Running Speech

Recognizing spoken language involves automatic activation of multiple candidate words. The process of selection between candidates is made more efficient by inhibition of embedded words (like egg in beg) that leave a portion of the input stranded (here, b). Results from European languages suggest that this inhibition occurs when consonants are stranded but not when syllables are stranded. The reason why leftover syllables do not lead to inhibition could be that in principle they might themselves be words; in European languages, a syllable can be a word. In Sesotho (a Bantu language), however, a single syllable cannot be a word. We report that in Sesotho, word recognition is inhibited by stranded consonants, but stranded monosyllables produce no more difficulty than stranded bisyllables (which could be Sesotho words). This finding suggests that the viability constraint which inhibits spurious embedded word candidates is not sensitive to language-specific word structure, but is universal.

[1]  D. Norris,et al.  The Possible-Word Constraint in the Segmentation of Continuous Speech , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Recognizing Spoken Words: The Neighborhood Activation Model , 1998, Ear and hearing.

[3]  B. Hayes Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies , 1995 .

[4]  D. Norris Shortlist: a connectionist model of continuous speech recognition , 1994, Cognition.

[5]  Anne Cutler,et al.  The Syllable's Role in the Segmentation of Stress Languages , 1997 .

[6]  K. Demuth,et al.  The prosodic structure of early words , 1996 .

[7]  P C Gordon,et al.  Lexical and prelexical influences on word segmentation: evidence from priming. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  J. Pind The Discovery of Spoken Language, Peter W. Jusczyk (Ed.). MIT Press (1997), ISBN 0 262 10058 4 , 1997 .

[9]  C. M. Doke,et al.  Textbook of Southern Sotho Grammar , 2000 .

[10]  Anne Cutler,et al.  The roll of the silly ball , 2001 .

[11]  Ulrich H. Frauenfelder,et al.  Boundaries versus Onsets in Syllabic Segmentation , 2001 .

[12]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Integrating Form and Meaning: A Distributed Model of Speech Perception. , 1997 .

[13]  J. McQueen Segmentation of Continuous Speech Using Phonotactics , 1998 .

[14]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Competition and segmentation in spoken word recognition , 1994, ICSLP.

[15]  Competition in spoken word recognition: Spotting words in other words , 1994 .

[16]  A. Cutler,et al.  Rhythmic Cues and Possible-Word Constraints in Japanese Speech Segmentation ☆ , 2001 .

[17]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The TRACE model of speech perception , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Scott Myers,et al.  Tone and the structure of words in Shona , 1987 .

[19]  T. A. Cartwright,et al.  Distributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation , 1996, Cognition.

[20]  K. Demuth Maturation and the Acquisition of the Sesotho Passive , 1989 .

[21]  S. Goldinger,et al.  Form-based priming in spoken word recognition: the roles of competition and bias. , 1992, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition.

[22]  Anne Cutler,et al.  The syllable's differing role in the segmentation of French and English. , 1986 .

[23]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Spotting (different kinds of) words in (different kinds of) context , 1998 .

[24]  Anne Cutler,et al.  The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access , 1988 .

[25]  Jonni Kanerva Focus and phrasing in Chicheŵa phonology , 1990 .

[26]  Cristina Burani,et al.  Word Identification in Fluent Speech , 1995 .

[27]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Language-universal constraints on speech segmentation , 2001 .

[28]  J. Mehler,et al.  Mora or syllable? Speech segmentation in Japanese , 1993 .