The influence of vocabulary size, phonotactic probability, and wordlikeness on nonword repetitions of children with and without specific language impairment.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] T. Campbell,et al. Lexical influences on nonword repetition , 1995, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[2] B. Munson,et al. Phonological pattern frequency and speech production in adults and children. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[3] B A Lewis,et al. Residual effects of preschool phonology disorders in grade school, adolescence, and adulthood. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.
[4] J. Windsor,et al. General slowing in language impairment: methodological considerations in testing the hypothesis. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[5] J. Edwards,et al. Nonword repetitions of children with specific language impairment: Exploration of some explanations for their inaccuracies , 1998, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[6] Mary E. Beckman,et al. Phonetic Interpretation Papers in Laboratory Phonology VI: Speech perception, well-formedness and the statistics of the lexicon , 2004 .
[7] E. Bates,et al. Continuity in lexical and morphological development: a test of the critical mass hypothesis , 1994, Journal of Child Language.
[8] Elizabeth Bates,et al. Books Recieved , 2022, CL.
[9] M. Beckman,et al. The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children's production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[10] Benjamin Munson,et al. Relationships between nonword repetition accuracy and other measures of linguistic development in children with phonological disorders. , 2005, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[11] T. Campbell,et al. Nonword repetition and child language impairment. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[12] Cynthia L Fisher,et al. Implicit Memory Support for Language Acquisition , 2001 .
[13] J. Windsor,et al. The search for common ground: Part II. Nonlinguistic performance by linguistically diverse learners. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[14] G. Conti-Ramsden. Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment (SLI). , 2003, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[15] S. Gathercole. Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords , 1995, Memory & cognition.
[16] G. Conti-Ramsden,et al. Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[17] A. Baddeley,et al. Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? ☆ , 1990 .
[18] J. Windsor,et al. The search for common ground: Part I. Lexical performance by linguistically diverse learners. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[19] J. Metsala,et al. Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: precursors to p , 1998 .
[20] David B Pisoni,et al. Perception of Wordlikeness: Effects of Segment Probability and Length on the Processing of Nonwords. , 2000, Journal of memory and language.
[21] C. Frankish,et al. Phonotactic influences on short-term memory. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[22] M L Rice,et al. Quick Incidental Learning (QUIL) of words by school-age children with and without SLI. , 1995, Journal of speech and hearing research.
[23] Irving Lorge,et al. Columbia mental maturity scale , 1954 .
[24] C Donlan,et al. Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[25] A. Baddeley,et al. The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names , 1990 .
[26] J B Tomblin,et al. Nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. , 2000, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[27] Catherine L. Rogers,et al. Final consonant discrimination in children: effects of phonological disorder, vocabulary size, and articulatory accuracy. , 2002, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[28] G. Conti-Ramsden,et al. Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). , 2001, International journal of language & communication disorders.
[29] J. Arena,et al. Expressive one-word picture vocabulary test , 1984 .
[30] C. Dollaghan,et al. Fast mapping in normal and language-impaired children. , 1987, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.
[31] E. Bates,et al. 2 ON THE INSEPARABILITY OF GRAMMAR AND THE LEXICON : EVIDENCE FROM ACQUISITION , APHASIA AND REAL-TIME PROCESSING , 1997 .
[32] Todd M. Bailey,et al. Determinants of wordlikeness: Phonotactics or lexical neighborhoods? , 2001 .
[33] Cleborne D. Maddux,et al. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III) , 1999 .
[34] P. Luce,et al. Probabilistic Phonotactics and Neighborhood Activation in Spoken Word Recognition , 1999 .
[35] A. Baddeley,et al. Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study , 1989 .
[36] David B. Pisoni,et al. Speech perception, word recognition and the structure of the lexicon , 1985, Speech Commun..
[37] H. Storkel. Restructuring of similarity neighbourhoods in the developing mental lexicon , 2002, Journal of Child Language.
[38] N. Gardner,et al. Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test , 1981 .
[39] Charles Hulme,et al. Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley , 1991, Applied Psycholinguistics.