Work first then play: Prior task difficulty increases motivation-related brain responses in a risk game

Task motivation depends on what we did before. A recent theory differentiates between tasks that we want to do and tasks that we have to do. After a have-to task, motivation shifts towards a want-to task. We measured this shift of motivation via brain responses to monetary feedback in a risk game that was used as want-to task in our study. We tested 20 healthy participants that were about 28 years old in a within-subjects design. Participants worked on a Stroop task (have-to task) or an easier version of the Stroop task as a control condition and played a risk game afterwards (want-to task). After the Stroop task, brain responses to monetary feedback in the risk game were larger compared to the easier control task, especially for feedback indicating higher monetary rewards. We conclude that higher amplitudes of feedback-related brain responses in the risk game reflect the shift of motivation after a have-to task towards a want-to task.

[1]  Rebecca G. Boswell,et al.  Self-Regulatory Depletion Enhances Neural Responses to Rewards and Impairs Top-Down Control , 2013, Psychological science.

[2]  M. Botvinick,et al.  A labor/leisure tradeoff in cognitive control. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback correct-related positivity: sensitivity of the event-related brain potential to unexpected positive feedback. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[4]  Scott E. Maxwell,et al.  Designing Experiments and Analyzing Data: A Model Comparison Perspective , 1990 .

[5]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  ERP correlates of feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence of response choice. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  P. Fischer,et al.  Ego Depletion Increases Risk-Taking , 2012, The Journal of social psychology.

[7]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  Can money enhance intrinsic motivation? A test of the content-consequence hypothesis. , 1975 .

[8]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Sensation Seeking And Risky Behavior , 2006 .

[9]  Till R. Schneider,et al.  Using ICA for the Analysis of Multi-Channel EEG Data , 2010 .

[10]  Xingyu Wang,et al.  A P300 Brain-Computer Interface Based on a Modification of the Mismatch Negativity Paradigm , 2015, Int. J. Neural Syst..

[11]  Y. Bin,et al.  The Strength Model of Self-Control , 2013 .

[12]  K. Vohs,et al.  What People Desire, Feel Conflicted About, and Try to Resist in Everyday Life , 2012, Psychological science.

[13]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  O. Wilhelm,et al.  Structure and Correlates of the German Version of the Brief UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scales , 2009 .

[15]  J. Montero-Marín,et al.  Too Much of a Good Thing: A Neuro-Dynamic Personality Model Explaining Engagement and Its Protective Inhibition , 2016 .

[16]  S. Whiteside,et al.  The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity , 2001 .

[17]  Johannes Hewig,et al.  Feedback-related potentials are sensitive to sequential order of decision outcomes in a gambling task. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[18]  Hiroaki Masaki,et al.  Affective-motivational influences on feedback-related ERPs in a gambling task , 2006, Brain Research.

[19]  P. Rainville,et al.  After‐effects of cognitive control on pain , 2013, European journal of pain.

[20]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  The role of outcome expectations in the generation of the feedback-related negativity. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[21]  J. Hewig,et al.  I'm too calm--let's take a risk! On the impact of state and trait arousal on risk taking. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[22]  R. Baumeister,et al.  High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. , 2004, Journal of personality.

[23]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[24]  Uta Sailer,et al.  Manipulation of feedback expectancy and valence induces negative and positive reward prediction error signals manifest in event-related brain potentials. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[25]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[26]  Wei Wu,et al.  Multimodal BCIs: Target Detection, Multidimensional Control, and Awareness Evaluation in Patients With Disorder of Consciousness , 2016, Proceedings of the IEEE.

[27]  G. Stemmler,et al.  Dopamine modulates frontomedial failure processing of agentic introverts versus extraverts in incentive contexts , 2013, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[28]  Johannes Hewig,et al.  I can't wait! Neural reward signals in impulsive individuals exaggerate the difference between immediate and future rewards. , 2017, Psychophysiology.

[29]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  Exercising self-control increases approach motivation. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  Jonathan Westley Peirce,et al.  Neuroinformatics Original Research Article Generating Stimuli for Neuroscience Using Psychopy , 2022 .

[31]  Michael Inzlicht,et al.  Running on empty: neural signals for self-control failure. , 2007, Psychological science.

[32]  G. Potts,et al.  Medial frontal event-related potentials and reward prediction: Do responses matter? , 2011, Brain and Cognition.

[33]  Mary E. Larimer,et al.  Exploring College Student Gambling Motivation , 2004, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[34]  Johannes Hewig,et al.  Decision-making in Blackjack: an electrophysiological analysis. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[36]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  M. Hagger,et al.  Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[38]  Peter Dalgaard,et al.  R Development Core Team (2010): R: A language and environment for statistical computing , 2010 .

[39]  Alexander Unger,et al.  Ego-Depletion and Risk Behavior: Too Exhausted to Take a Risk , 2011 .

[40]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[41]  Ego Depletion , 2019, The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation.

[42]  A. Sanfey,et al.  Independent Coding of Reward Magnitude and Valence in the Human Brain , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  David Cucurell,et al.  On the number of trials needed for a stable feedback-related negativity. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[44]  Scott A. Huettel,et al.  Rapid Electrophysiological Brain Responses are Influenced by Both Valence and Magnitude of Monetary Rewards , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Oliver Dickhäuser,et al.  Messung dispositioneller Selbstkontroll-Kapazität , 2009 .

[46]  B. Rasch,et al.  Suppressing Emotions Impairs Subsequent Stroop Performance and Reduces Prefrontal Brain Activation , 2013, PloS one.

[47]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  D. Tucker,et al.  Scalp electrode impedance, infection risk, and EEG data quality , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[49]  M. Coles,et al.  The influence of the magnitude, probability, and valence of potential wins and losses on the amplitude of the feedback negativity. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[50]  J. Hewig,et al.  Paying Out One or All Trials: A Behavioral Economic Evaluation of Payment Methods in a Prototypical Risky Decision Study , 2015 .