Preparatory encoding of the fine scale of human spatial attention

Our attentional focus is constantly shifting: In one moment, our attention may be intently concentrated on a specific spot, whereas in another moment we might spread our attention more broadly. Although much is known about the mechanisms by which we shift our visual attention from place to place, relatively little is known about how we shift the aperture of attention from more narrowly to more broadly focused. Here we introduce a novel attentional distribution task to examine the neural mechanisms underlying this process. In this task, participants are presented with an informative cue that indicates the location of an upcoming target. This cue can be perfectly predictive of the exact target location, or it can indicate—with varying degrees of certainty—approximately where the target might appear. This cue is followed by a preparatory period in which there is nothing on the screen except a central fixation cross. Using scalp EEG, we examined neural activity during this preparatory period. We find that, with decreasing certainty regarding the precise location of the impending target, participant RTs increased whereas target identification accuracy decreased. Additionally, the multivariate pattern of preparatory period visual cortical alpha (8–12 Hz) activity encoded attentional distribution. This alpha encoding was predictive of behavioral accuracy and RT nearly 1 sec later. These results offer insight into the neural mechanisms underlying how we use information to guide our attentional distribution and how that influences behavior.

[1]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  D. Heeger,et al.  The Normalization Model of Attention , 2009, Neuron.

[3]  H. Jasper,et al.  Electrocorticograms in man: Effect of voluntary movement upon the electrical activity of the precentral gyrus , 1949 .

[4]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The effect of cueing on unilateral neglect , 1983, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  John T. Serences,et al.  Attention modulates spatial priority maps in the human occipital, parietal and frontal cortices , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Spatial gradients of visual attention: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  M. Leek Adaptive procedures in psychophysical research , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  Edward F. Ester,et al.  Parietal and Frontal Cortex Encode Stimulus-Specific Mnemonic Representations during Visual Working Memory , 2015, Neuron.

[10]  E. Vogel,et al.  Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  E. Vogel,et al.  Neural Measures of Individual Differences in Selecting and Tracking Multiple Moving Objects , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  J. Palva,et al.  New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[13]  George A. Alvarez,et al.  The Role of the Parietal Lobe in Visual Extinction Studied with Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Bradley Voytek,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia contributions to visual working memory , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  A. Nobre,et al.  Indexing the graded allocation of visuospatial attention using anticipatory alpha oscillations , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal modulation of visual processing in humans , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Jeremy Freeman,et al.  Orientation Decoding Depends on Maps, Not Columns , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  G. Shulman,et al.  Spatial determinants of the distribution of attention , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  Bradley R. Postle,et al.  Decoding and Reconstructing the Focus of Spatial Attention from the Topography of Alpha-band Oscillations , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Linyan Xue,et al.  Using spatial uncertainty to manipulate the size of the attention focus , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[21]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Tracking multiple targets with multifocal attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  Jeremy Freeman,et al.  Motion Direction Biases and Decoding in Human Visual Cortex , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  R. Knight,et al.  Shifts in Gamma Phase–Amplitude Coupling Frequency from Theta to Alpha Over Posterior Cortex During Visual Tasks , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[25]  Bradley Voytek,et al.  Alpha phase dynamics predict age-related visual working memory decline , 2016, NeuroImage.

[26]  Andrew Hollingworth,et al.  The spatial distribution of attention within and across objects. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[28]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  John T. Serences,et al.  Visual attention mitigates information loss in small- and large-scale neural codes , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  G. Alvarez,et al.  Spatial separation between targets constrains maintenance of attention on multiple objects , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  G. Woodman,et al.  Event-related potential studies of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  G R Mangun,et al.  Spatial distribution of visual attention: Perceptual sensitivity and response latency , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Age-Related Changes in 1/f Neural Electrophysiological Noise , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Blind separation and blind deconvolution: an information-theoretic approach , 1995, 1995 International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing.

[35]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Frontoparietal Cortex Controls Spatial Attention through Modulation of Anticipatory Alpha Rhythms , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  O. Jensen,et al.  Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[37]  A. Nobre,et al.  Alpha Oscillations Related to Anticipatory Attention Follow Temporal Expectations , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.