Tactile Apparent Motion on the Torso Modulates Perceived Forward Self-Motion Velocity

The present study investigated whether a tactile flow created by a matrix of vibrators in a seat pan simultaneously presented with an optical flow in peripheral vision enhances the perceived forward velocity of self-motion. A brief tactile motion stimulus consisted of four successive rows of vibration, and the interstimulus onset between the tactile rows was varied to change the velocity of the tactile motion. The results show that the forward velocity of self-motion is significantly overestimated for rapid tactile flows and underestimated for slow ones, compared with optical flow alone or non-motion vibrotactile stimulation conditions. In addition, the effect with a temporal tactile rhythm without changing the stimulus location was smaller than that with spatiotemporal tactile motion, with the interstimulus onset interval to elicit a clear sensation of tactile apparent motion. These findings suggest that spatiotemporal tactile motion is effective in inducing a change in the perceived forward velocity of self-motion.

[1]  H. E. Burtt Tactual illusions of movement , 1917 .

[2]  J. H. Killebrew,et al.  Influence of visual motion on tactile motion perception. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Carl E. Sherrick,et al.  Apparent haptic movement , 1966 .

[4]  M. Kuefer Psychophysics Introduction To Its Perceptual Neural And Social Prospects , 2016 .

[5]  Daniel J. Hannon,et al.  Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.

[6]  Ali Israr,et al.  Tactile brush: drawing on skin with a tactile grid display , 2011, CHI.

[7]  Raymond J Kiefer,et al.  Direction coding using a tactile chair. , 2009, Applied ergonomics.

[8]  Bernhard E. Riecke,et al.  Moving sounds enhance the visually-induced self-motion illusion (circular vection) in virtual reality , 2009, TAP.

[9]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Towards lean and elegant self-motion simulation in virtual reality , 2005, IEEE Proceedings. VR 2005. Virtual Reality, 2005..

[10]  I. Howard,et al.  Vection: The Contributions of Absolute and Relative Visual Motion , 1994, Perception.

[11]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[12]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Planar directional contributions to optic flow responses in MST neurons. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Brian M. Kleiner,et al.  Toward Developing an Approach for Alerting Drivers to the Direction of a Crash Threat , 2007, Hum. Factors.

[14]  Felix Blankenburg,et al.  Tactile and visual motion direction processing in hMT+/V5 , 2014, NeuroImage.

[15]  Luca Turchet,et al.  Haptically Induced Illusory Self-motion and the Influence of Context of Motion , 2012, EuroHaptics.

[16]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  Feeling the Beat: Premotor and Striatal Interactions in Musicians and Nonmusicians during Beat Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  David M. Eagleman,et al.  Using space and time to encode vibrotactile information: toward an estimate of the skin’s achievable throughput , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  S. Coren,et al.  In Sensation and perception , 1979 .

[19]  Shuichi Sakamoto,et al.  The effects of linearly moving sound images on self-motion perception , 2004 .

[20]  Monica Gori,et al.  Cross-Sensory Facilitation Reveals Neural Interactions between Visual and Tactile Motion in Humans , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[21]  Martin V. Butz,et al.  Remapping motion across modalities: tactile rotations influence visual motion judgments , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  J. Kirman,et al.  Tactile apparent movement: The effects of interstimulus onset interval and stimulus duration , 1974 .

[23]  Geraint Rees,et al.  The Cutaneous Rabbit Illusion Affects Human Primary Sensory Cortex Somatotopically , 2006, PLoS biology.

[24]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  Touching Motion: rTMS on the Human Middle Temporal Complex Interferes with Tactile Speed Perception , 2012, Brain Topography.

[25]  Vincent Hayward,et al.  Motion Aftereffects Transfer between Touch and Vision , 2009, Current Biology.

[26]  Shoji Sunaga,et al.  Consistent Air Flow to the Face Facilitates Vection , 2011, Perception.

[27]  Anatole Lécuyer,et al.  Can haptic feedback improve the perception of self-motion in virtual reality? , 2004, 12th International Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, 2004. HAPTICS '04. Proceedings..

[28]  M S Redfern,et al.  Postural sway of patients with vestibular disorders during optic flow. , 1994, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[29]  F H Previc,et al.  A comparison of the latencies of visually induced postural change and self-motion perception. , 1990, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[30]  Daniela Bonino,et al.  Functional inhibition of the human middle temporal cortex affects non-visual motion perception: a repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation study during tactile speed discrimination , 2011, Experimental biology and medicine.

[31]  François Klam,et al.  ã Federation of European Neuroscience Societies Visual±vestibular interactive responses in the macaque ventral intraparietal area (VIP) , 2022 .

[32]  Felix Blankenburg,et al.  Tactile Motion and Pattern Processing Assessed with High-Field fMRI , 2011, PloS one.

[33]  Tomohiro Amemiya,et al.  Tactile duration compression by vibrotactile adaptation , 2010, Neuroreport.

[34]  Matthew Brett,et al.  Rhythm and Beat Perception in Motor Areas of the Brain , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Representation of Vestibular and Visual Cues to Self-Motion in Ventral Intraparietal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Anatole Lécuyer,et al.  Haptic motion: Improving sensation of self-motion in virtual worlds with force feedback , 2014, 2014 IEEE Haptics Symposium (HAPTICS).

[37]  Ryan M. Traylor,et al.  A Haptic Back Display for Attentional and Directional Cueing , 2003 .

[38]  Vincent Hayward,et al.  Vibrotactile inputs to the feet can modulate vection , 2013, 2013 World Haptics Conference (WHC).

[39]  Yasushi Ikei,et al.  Tactile flow on seat pan modulates perceived forward velocity , 2013, 2013 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI).

[40]  F. A. Geldard,et al.  The Cutaneous "Rabbit": A Perceptual Illusion , 1972, Science.

[41]  Tao Zhang,et al.  Parietal Area VIP Causally Influences Heading Perception during Pursuit Eye Movements , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[43]  Jan B. F. van Erp,et al.  A Tactile Seat for Direction Coding in Car Driving: Field Evaluation , 2009, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.