Affective decision-making predictive of Chinese adolescent drinking behaviors

Abstract The goal of the current investigation was to address whether affective decision making would serve as a unique neuropsychological marker to predict drinking behaviors among adolescents. We conducted a longitudinal study of 181 Chinese adolescents in Chengdu city, China. In their 10th grade (ages 15–16), these adolescents were tested for their affective decision-making ability using the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) and working memory capacity using the Self-Ordered Pointing Test. Self-report questionnaires were used to assess academic performance and drinking behaviors. At 1-year follow-up, questionnaires were completed to assess drinking behaviors, and the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale was used to examine four dimensions of impulsivity: urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance, and sensation seeking. Results indicated that those adolescents who progressed to binge drinking or exhibited consistent binge drinking not only performed poorly on the IGT but also scored significantly higher in urgency compared to those who never or occasionally drank. Moreover, better IGT scores predicted fewer drinking problems and fewer drinks 1 year later after controlling for demographic variables, the previous drinking behaviors, working memory, and impulsivity. These findings suggest that deficits in affective decision making may be important independent determinants of compulsive drinking and potentially addictive behavior in adolescents. (JINS, 2009, 15, 547–557.)

[1]  Jerry L. Grenard,et al.  Affective decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in 10th grade Chinese adolescent binge drinkers , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  Exploring the neurological substrate of emotional and social intelligence. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  Maureen Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Working memory and aging: A cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis using a self-ordered pointing task , 2004, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[4]  Catalina J. Hooper,et al.  Adolescents' performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: implications for the development of decision making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[5]  L. Kann,et al.  Overview of the Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System. , 1993, Public health reports.

[6]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Decision-making and addiction (part I): impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of reward prediction error recorded in the human prefrontal cortex. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  A. Bechara The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[10]  L. Johnston,et al.  Tobacco, alcohol, and illicit drug use: racial and ethnic differences among U.S. high school seniors, 1976-2000. , 2002, Public health reports.

[11]  S. Funahashi Prefrontal cortex and working memory processes , 2006, Neuroscience.

[12]  Jerry L. Grenard,et al.  Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. , 2008, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[13]  P. Gallaher,et al.  Tobacco use among youth and adults in Mainland China: the China Seven Cities Study. , 2006, Public health.

[14]  Patrick C. Kyllonen,et al.  Working memory is (almost) perfectly predicted by g , 2004 .

[15]  Deborah A. Yurgelun-Todd,et al.  Neural correlates of emotional intelligence in adolescent children , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[16]  M. McGue,et al.  A Developmental Perspective on Alcohol and Youths 16 to 20 Years of Age , 2008, Pediatrics.

[17]  J. Giedd Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Adolescent Brain , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  T. Joiner,et al.  The role of urgency in maladaptive behaviors. , 2007, Behaviour research and therapy.

[19]  L. Kirisci,et al.  Association between hyperactivity and executive cognitive functioning in childhood and substance use in early adolescence. , 1999, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[20]  E. Rolls,et al.  Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Chengye Ji,et al.  Relationship of binge drinking and other health-compromising behaviors among urban adolescents in China. , 2006, The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine.

[22]  A. Damasio,et al.  Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  H. White,et al.  Towards the assessment of adolescent problem drinking. , 1989, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[24]  S. Whiteside,et al.  The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity , 2001 .

[25]  M Petrides,et al.  Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex in the monkey , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  Philip David Zelazo,et al.  Development of “hot” executive function: The children’s gambling task , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[27]  B. Milner,et al.  Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Jerry L. Grenard,et al.  Working memory capacity moderates the predictive effects of drug-related associations on substance use. , 2008, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[29]  J. Bachman,et al.  Validity of self-reports in student-based studies on minority populations: issues and concerns. , 1993, NIDA research monograph.

[30]  R. K. Price,et al.  Alcohol, tobacco, and other drug use among Asian American and Pacific Islander Adolescents in California and Hawaii. , 2004, Addictive behaviors.

[31]  Andrea Stocco,et al.  Implicit emotional biases in decision making: The case of the Iowa Gambling Task , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[32]  P. Holowaty,et al.  Alcohol use beliefs and behaviors among high school students. , 1999, The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine.

[33]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[34]  Robert A Zucker,et al.  Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. , 2006, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[35]  K. Sher,et al.  Decision making and binge drinking: a longitudinal study. , 2007, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[36]  E. Crone,et al.  Developmental Changes in Real Life Decision Making: Performance on a Gambling Task Previously Shown to Depend on the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex , 2004, Developmental neuropsychology.

[37]  W. Overman,et al.  Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Patrick B. Johnson,et al.  The Relationship Between Adolescent Smoking and Drinking and Likelihood Estimates of Illicit Drug Use , 2000, Journal of addictive diseases.

[39]  P. Eling,et al.  Working memory in schizophrenia: A systematic study of specific modalities and processes , 2011, Psychiatry Research.

[40]  M. Dixon,et al.  Affective guidance in the Iowa gambling task , 2006, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  M McGue,et al.  Behavioral disinhibition and the development of substance-use disorders: Findings from the Minnesota Twin Family Study , 1999, Development and Psychopathology.

[42]  D L Braff,et al.  Working memory in schizophrenia: transient "online" storage versus executive functioning. , 2001, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[43]  J. Towse,et al.  Individual differences in working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[44]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.