Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control, and working memory

Cognitive control has often been associated with activations of middorsolateral prefrontal cortex. However, recent evidence highlights the importance of a more posterior frontolateral region around the junction of the inferior frontal sulcus and the inferior precentral sulcus (the inferior frontal junction area, IFJ). In the present experiment, we investigated the involvement of the IFJ in a task-switching paradigm, a manual Stroop task, and a verbal n-back task in a within-session within-group design. After computing contrasts for the individual tasks, the resulting z maps were overlaid to identify areas commonly activated by these tasks. Common activations were found in the IFJ, in the pre-SMA extending into mesial BA 8, in the middle frontal gyrus bordering the inferior frontal sulcus, in the anterior insula, and in parietal and thalamic regions. These results indicate the existence of a network of prefrontal, parietal, and subcortical regions mediating cognitive control in task coordination, interference control, and working memory. In particular, the results provide evidence for the assumption that, in the frontolateral cortex, not only the middorsolateral region but also the IFJ plays an important role in cognitive control.

[1]  Earl K. Miller,et al.  Cognitive focus through adaptive neural coding in the primate prefrontal cortex , 2002 .

[2]  E. Miller,et al.  Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  R. Buckner,et al.  Transient Activation during Block Transition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[4]  M. Brass,et al.  A receptor- and cytoarchitectonic correlate of the functionally defined inferior-frontal junction area , 2004 .

[5]  Bruce L. Miller Principles of Frontal Lobe Function , 2003 .

[6]  Lars Nyberg,et al.  Seeing the Forest Through the Trees : The Cross-Function Approach to Imaging Cognition , 2002 .

[7]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  N. Cohen,et al.  The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[9]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[10]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry in human lateral prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited , 1995, NeuroImage.

[12]  G. Calvert Crossmodal processing in the human brain: insights from functional neuroimaging studies. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  Norihiro Sadato,et al.  Neuronal substrates participating in attentional set-shifting of rules for visually guided motor selection: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation , 1999, Neuroscience Research.

[14]  Thomas A Hammeke,et al.  Neural basis of the Stroop interference task: Response competition or selective attention? , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[15]  D. A. Taylor,et al.  The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  M. Brass,et al.  Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Empirical Analyses of BOLD fMRI Statistics , 1997, NeuroImage.

[19]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Contribution of working memory to transient activation in human inferior prefrontal cortex during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[21]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[22]  K. Berman,et al.  Fractionating the neural substrate of cognitive control processes , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[24]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[25]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Brain mapping : the systems , 2000 .

[26]  D. Stuss,et al.  Principles of frontal lobe function , 2002 .

[27]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[28]  H. Fukuyama,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of attentional control within the human prefrontal cortex. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited—Again , 1995, NeuroImage.

[30]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  S. Bookheimer,et al.  Dissociating Neural Mechanisms of Temporal Sequencing and Processing Phonemes , 2003, Neuron.

[32]  Alice Mado Proverbio,et al.  The cognitive electrophysiology of mind and brain , 2003 .

[33]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  PET Evidence for an Amodal Verbal Working Memory System , 1996, NeuroImage.

[34]  Stefan Pollmann,et al.  Event‐related fMRI: Comparison of conditions with varying BOLD overlap , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[35]  M. Rushworth,et al.  The functional organization of the lateral frontal cortex: conjecture or conjuncture in the electrophysiology literature? , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Jane E Joseph,et al.  Hypothesis testing for selective, differential, and conjoined brain activation , 2002, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[37]  G. Lohmann,et al.  Color-Word Matching Stroop Task: Separating Interference and Response Conflict , 2001, NeuroImage.

[38]  D. Schacter,et al.  Prefrontal Contributions to Executive Control: fMRI Evidence for Functional Distinctions within Lateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2001, NeuroImage.

[39]  S. Petersen,et al.  Hemispheric Specialization in Human Dorsal Frontal Cortex and Medial Temporal Lobe for Verbal and Nonverbal Memory Encoding , 1998, Neuron.

[40]  Robin M. Chan,et al.  Working memory for complex figures: an fMRI comparison of letter and fractal n-back tasks. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[41]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Neuroanatomic Overlap of Working Memory and Spatial Attention Networks: A Functional MRI Comparison within Subjects , 1999, NeuroImage.

[42]  Jin Fan,et al.  Cognitive and Brain Consequences of Conflict , 2003, NeuroImage.

[43]  E. Miller,et al.  From rule to response: neuronal processes in the premotor and prefrontal cortex. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  S. Wise,et al.  Arbitrary associations between antecedents and actions , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[45]  Michael Petrides,et al.  6 – Mapping Prefrontal Cortical Systems for the Control of Cognition , 2000 .

[46]  A. Dove,et al.  Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[47]  Vinod Goel,et al.  Reciprocal neural response within lateral and ventral medial prefrontal cortex during hot and cold reasoning , 2003, NeuroImage.

[48]  K. A. Hadland,et al.  Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  M. Petrides,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct Neural Circuits Participating in Different Stages of the Task Identified by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[51]  David Caplan,et al.  Cognitive conjunction and cognitive functions , 2004, NeuroImage.

[52]  G. R. Mangun,et al.  A Role for Top-Down Attentional Orienting during Interference between Global and Local Aspects of Hierarchical Stimuli , 2002, NeuroImage.

[53]  G Lohmann,et al.  LIPSIA--a new software system for the evaluation of functional magnetic resonance images of the human brain. , 2001, Computerized medical imaging and graphics : the official journal of the Computerized Medical Imaging Society.

[54]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[55]  M. Brass,et al.  Advance preparation and stimulus-induced interference in cued task switching: further insights from BOLD fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[58]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Verbal Working Memory Load Affects Regional Brain Activation as Measured by PET , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[60]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cognitive Conjunction: A New Approach to Brain Activation Experiments , 1997, NeuroImage.

[61]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[62]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Empirical Analyses of BOLD fMRI Statistics , 1997, NeuroImage.

[63]  J. Ashe,et al.  The Effect of Stimulus–Response Compatibility on Cortical Motor Activation , 2001, NeuroImage.