Reading, arithmetic, and task orientation—How are they related?

A sample of 60 children in Grade 3 was followed over one year. In the first year, an extensive battery of assessments was used including aspects of reading, arithmetic, and working memory. Teachers rated the children on 7-point scales on various motivational dimensions summarized to a total score tentatively called task orientation. In the follow-up assessment one year later, the testing and teacher ratings were repeated. The cross-sectional correlations between reading, arithmetic, and task orientation were all high (about +.70). The high correlation between reading and arithmetic decreased significantly when task orientation was partialed out, and it was further reduced when working memory as assessed by backward digit span was added to the controlling factors. Also, teacher ratings of cognitive ability and language development accounted for some of the common variance between reading and arithmetic. The correlation between task orientation and school achievement cannot be causally interpreted in cross-sectional designs. Some support for a “causal” hypothesis, however, was obtained in crosslagged correlation analyses indicating that task orientation in Grade 3 may have a causal impact on the level of performance in reading, and in arithmetic in Grade 4. Most likely, however, there is also a reciprocal relationship.

[1]  D G Gadian,et al.  Calculation difficulties in children of very low birthweight: a neural correlate. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  C. Francks,et al.  Genes, cognition and dyslexia: learning to read the genome , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  S. Pickering,et al.  Research Section: Working memory deficits in children with special educational needs , 2003 .

[4]  E. Deci,et al.  Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations: Classic Definitions and New Directions. , 2000, Contemporary educational psychology.

[5]  B. S. Randhawa,et al.  Role of Mathematics Self-Efficacy in the Structural Model of Mathematics Achievement. , 1993 .

[6]  Kaisa Aunola,et al.  Developmental dynamics between mathematical performance, task motivation, and teachers' goals during the transition to primary school. , 2006, The British journal of educational psychology.

[7]  Thomas A Zeffiro,et al.  Development of neural mechanisms for reading , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  A. E. Gottfried Academic intrinsic motivation in young elementary school children. , 1990 .

[9]  Carol S. Dweck,et al.  Determinants of academic achievement: The interaction of children's achievement orientations with skill area. , 1984 .

[10]  Jari-Erik Nurmi,et al.  The role of task-avoidant and task-focused behaviors in the development of reading and mathematical skills during the first school year: A cross-lagged longitudinal study. , 2000 .

[11]  Kirby Deater-Deckard,et al.  A cross-sectional behavioral genetic analysis of task persistence in the transition to middle childhood. , 2005, Developmental science.

[12]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  The cognitive correlates of third-grade skill in arithmetic, algorithmic computation, and arithmetic word problems , 2006 .

[13]  Carole A. Ames,et al.  Achievement Goals in the Classroom: Students' Learning Strategies and Motivation Processes , 1988 .

[14]  M. Craske Learned helplessness, self-worth motivation and attribution retraining for primary school children. , 1988, The British journal of educational psychology.

[15]  John G. Nicholls,et al.  Individual differences in academic motivation: Perceived ability, goals, beliefs, and values , 1989 .

[16]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Dyscalculia Screener, London: NFER-Nelson (software and manual). , 2003 .

[17]  Charles Hulme,et al.  The cognitive foundations of reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year-olds. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[18]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The phonological loop as a language learning device. , 1998, Psychological review.

[19]  P. Pintrich,et al.  Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. , 1990 .

[20]  S. Pickering,et al.  Assessment of Working Memory in Six- and Seven-Year-Old Children. , 2000 .

[21]  R. Plomin,et al.  Generalist genes and learning disabilities. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  S. Gathercole,et al.  Working memory in children with reading disabilities. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  C. Dweck,et al.  An Analysis of Learned Helplessness: Continuous Changes in Performance, Strategy, and Achievement Cognitions Following Failure. , 1978 .

[24]  Carole A. Ames Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. , 1992 .

[25]  D. Geary Mathematics and Learning Disabilities , 2004, Journal of learning disabilities.

[26]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students , 2004, Cognition.

[27]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[28]  Herbert J. Walberg,et al.  Motivation and Achievement: A Quantitative Synthesis , 1979 .

[29]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Arithmetic and the Brain This Review Comes from a Themed Issue on Cognitive Neuroscience Edited the Intraparietal Sulcus and Number Sense Number Sense in the Animal Brain , 2022 .

[30]  D. Fuchs,et al.  Predicting LD on the Basis of Motivation, Metacognition, and Psychopathology , 2006, Journal of learning disabilities.

[31]  J. Bisanz,et al.  Representation and working memory in early arithmetic. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[32]  Elizabeth J. Newton,et al.  Number skills and knowledge in children with specific language impairment , 2005 .

[33]  G J Hitch,et al.  The prevalence of specific arithmetic difficulties and specific reading difficulties in 9- to 10-year-old boys and girls. , 1994, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[34]  R. Wagner,et al.  The relations between phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences in mathematical computation skills: a longitudinal study from second to fifth grades. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[35]  R Plomin,et al.  'Generalist genes' and mathematics in 7-year-old twins. , 2005, Intelligence.