The evolution of courtship rituals in monogamous species

In this paper we propose an alternative explanation for the evolution of courtship rituals in monogamous species. We demonstrate, using computer simulations, how male courtship might develop as males exploit response biases in females to manipulate the female into starting reproduction before she has been able to assess the male’s intentions. In our coevolutionary simulations, a recurrent, artificial neural network is used to model the female recognition mechanism, while the displaying male is represented by a sequence of signals. Our particular model situation is just one example of how a reproductive conflict could result in the evolution of ritualized displays in monogamous species. Since reproductive conflicts occur even after pair formations, the explanation we propose may also apply to rituals that occur after pair formation. Key words: artificial neural networks, courtship, ritualization, coyness, manipulation, mate choice, monogamy, reproductive conflict, receiver bias, sexual selection. [Behav Ecol 11:405–410 (2000)] A variety of signals and behaviors are used in interactions with the opposite sex. In monogamous species these rituals may occur over an extended period of time. Many of these interactions are identified as courtship and are today often regarded as means of assessing partner quality (Andersson, 1994; Trivers, 1972). For instance, partner quality may be revealed because high-quality males can afford to use more elaborate displays or because a direct relationship exists between quality and performance (see, e.g., Andersson, 1994). Older ethological explanations of courtship rituals have stressed the importance of stimulation and cooperation, rather than choice of partner. For instance, Huxley (1914: 516) proposed that these ritualized displays serve ‘‘to keep the two birds of a pair together, and to keep them constant to each other’’ (see also Armstrong, 1963). A similar idea is that courtship rituals provide necessary stimulation and that they coordinate the reproductive physiology of the male and the female (Bastock, 1967; Lehrman, 1959, 1964). The elaboration of displays was considered to reduce ambiguity (Cullen, 1966). In this paper we describe a new hypothesis for the evolution of rituals within a monogamous pair. Here the courtship display is not informative to the female apart from indicating the presence of a male of her own species. Our hypothesis accords with classical ethology (theories of ritualization; see, e.g., EiblEibesfeldt, 1975) insofar as the emphasis is on stimulation, but it rests on reproductive conflict and manipulation rather than on cooperation. In monogamous species there are a variety of such reproductive conflicts, many concerning parental duties. To illustrate how this can lead to evolution of elaborated display and manipulations, we developed a model. Consider a female that has to find a partner in an environment of both faithful and philandering males. These males differ in the amount of parental care that they provide. Males are eager to court any female and start reproduction as soon as they meet a female. Females, on the other hand, might benefit by staying coy and evaluating the intention of a potential partner before actual reproduction, even if this is associated with some costs (Wachtmeister and Enquist, 1999). Our suggestion is that

[1]  P. O'donald Theoretical aspects of sexual selection. , 1977, Theoretical population biology.

[2]  Magnus Enquist,et al.  The Evolution of Female Coyness – Trading Time for Information , 1999 .

[3]  D C Krakauer,et al.  The evolution of exploitation and honesty in animal communication: a model using artificial neural networks. , 1995, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[4]  Anders Krogh,et al.  Introduction to the theory of neural computation , 1994, The advanced book program.

[5]  Magnus Enquist,et al.  Symmetry, beauty and evolution , 1994, Nature.

[6]  A Arak,et al.  Conflict, receiver bias and the evolution of signal form. , 1995, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[7]  E. A. Armstrong A study of bird song , 1964 .

[8]  S. Huxley 33. The Courtship - habits * of the Great Crested Grrebe (Podiceps cristatus); with an addition to the Theory of Sexual Selection. , 1914 .

[9]  M. Enquist,et al.  Selection of exaggerated male traits by female aesthetic senses , 1993, Nature.

[10]  R. Johnstone Female preference for symmetrical males as a by-product of selection for mate recognition , 1994, Nature.

[11]  J. M. Cullen,et al.  E. Ritualization of animal activities in relation to phylogeny, speciation and ecology: Reduction of ambiguity through ritualization , 1966, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences.

[12]  W. Rice,et al.  PERSPECTIVE: CHASE‐AWAY SEXUAL SELECTION: ANTAGONISTIC SEDUCTION VERSUS RESISTANCE , 1998, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[13]  J. Lifjeld,et al.  Polygyny in the pied flycatcher, Ficedula hypoleuca: a test of the deception hypothesis , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[14]  William Rowan,et al.  The Study of Instinct , 1953 .

[15]  A. Zahavi Mate selection-a selection for a handicap. , 1975, Journal of theoretical biology.

[16]  M. Ryan,et al.  Neural networks predict response biases of female túngara frogs , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  A. Grafen Biological signals as handicaps. , 1990, Journal of theoretical biology.

[18]  A. Zahavi The cost of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle). , 1977, Journal of theoretical biology.

[19]  David W. Johnston,et al.  Bird Display and Behaviour , 1965 .

[20]  Alan Hutchinson,et al.  Algorithmic Learning , 1994 .

[21]  T. Slagsvold,et al.  Male polyterritoriality and female-female aggression in pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  M. Ryan Sexual selection, receiver biases, and the evolution of sex differences. , 1998, Science.

[23]  A. Basolo Female Preference Predates the Evolution of the Sword in Swordtail Fish , 1990, Science.

[24]  T. Guilford,et al.  An exaggerated preference for simple neural network models of signal evolution? , 1995, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  D. Lehrman THE REPRODUCTIVE BEHAVIOR OF RING DOVES. , 1964, Scientific American.

[26]  N. Tinbergen,et al.  The Study of Instinct , 1953 .

[27]  J. Staddon A Note on the Evolutionary Significance of "Supernormal" Stimuli , 1975, The American Naturalist.

[28]  Rufus A. Johnstone,et al.  Generalization and the evolution of symmetry preferences , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  S. Ghirlanda,et al.  Artificial neural networks as models of stimulus control , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  R. Trivers Parental investment and sexual selection , 1972 .

[31]  R. Palmer,et al.  Introduction to the theory of neural computation , 1994, The advanced book program.

[32]  N. Mackintosh The psychology of animal learning , 1974 .

[33]  M. Enquist,et al.  Hidden preferences and the evolution of signals , 1993 .

[34]  O. Leimar,et al.  Evolutionary Stability of Aposematic Coloration and Prey Unprofitability: A Theoretical Analysis , 1986, The American Naturalist.

[35]  J. Huxley Courtship Activities in the Red-throated Diver (Colymbus stellatus Pontopp.); together with a discussion of the Evolution of Courtship in Birds , 1923 .

[36]  M. Ryan,et al.  Sexual selection for sensory exploitation in the frog Physalaemus pustulosus , 1990, Nature.

[37]  M. Enquist,et al.  Darwin’s principle of antithesis revisited: a role for perceptual biases in the evolution of intraspecific signals , 1995, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.