Social Sampling Explains Apparent Biases in Judgments of Social Environments
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] John R. Chambers,et al. Biases in social comparative judgments: the role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.
[2] James M. Olson,et al. Better, Stronger, Faster: Self-Serving Judgment, Affect Regulation, and the Optimal Vigilance Hypothesis , 2007, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[3] E. Brunswik. Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. , 1955, Psychological review.
[4] J. Bethlehem. Weighting nonresponse adjustments based on auxiliary information , 2002 .
[5] N. Chater,et al. The probabilistic mind: prospects for Bayesian cognitive science , 2008 .
[6] K Fiedler. Beware of samples! A cognitive-ecological sampling approach to judgment biases. , 2000, Psychological review.
[7] Jay W. Pope,et al. False Consensus Effect , 2013 .
[8] Jerker Denrell,et al. Why most people disapprove of me: experience sampling in impression formation. , 2005, Psychological review.
[9] B. Fischhoff,et al. Judged frequency of lethal events , 1978 .
[10] Richard P. Larrick,et al. Skilled or unskilled, but still unaware of it: how perceptions of difficulty drive miscalibration in relative comparisons. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[11] M. L. Klotz,et al. Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. , 1995 .
[12] Shelley E. Taylor,et al. Theory and Research Concerning Social Comparisons of Personal Attributes , 2001 .
[13] Joachim Krueger,et al. Unskilled, unaware, or both? The better-than-average heuristic and statistical regression predict errors in estimates of own performance. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[14] L. Ross,et al. The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes , 1977 .
[15] Deborah A Small,et al. Error and bias in comparative judgment: on being both better and worse than we think we are. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[16] Ulrike Hahn,et al. Unrealistic optimism about future life events: a cautionary note. , 2011, Psychological review.
[17] Y. Klar,et al. No one in my group can be below the group's average: a robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous peers. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[18] P. Juslin,et al. Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: a critical examination of the hard-easy effect. , 2000, Psychological review.
[19] D. Moore,et al. The trouble with overconfidence. , 2008, Psychological review.
[20] M. McPherson,et al. Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks , 2001 .
[21] Robyn M. Dawes,et al. The False Consensus Effect and Overconfidence: Flaws in Judgment or Flaws in How We Study Judgment? , 1996 .
[22] Klaus Fiedler,et al. Taking the interface between mind and environment seriously , 2006 .
[23] Jerry Suls,et al. Social Comparison: Why, With Whom, and With What Effect? , 2002 .
[24] R. Nisbett,et al. Perception of social distributions. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[25] Klaus Fiedler,et al. Information Sampling and Adaptive Cognition , 2005 .
[26] D. Moore. When good = better than average , 2007, Judgment and Decision Making.
[27] Pancultural self-enhancement. , 2003 .
[28] R. Hertwig,et al. Judgments of risk frequencies: tests of possible cognitive mechanisms. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[29] J. Kruger,et al. Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[30] P. Todd,et al. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart , 1999 .
[31] J. Kruger. Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[32] L. Festinger. A Theory of Social Comparison Processes , 1954 .
[33] Thorsten Pachur,et al. The Social Circle Heuristic: Fast and Frugal Decisions Based on Small Samples , 2005 .
[34] D. Funder,et al. Towards a balanced social psychology: Causes, consequences, and cures for the problem-seeking approach to social behavior and cognition , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[35] Olesya Govorun,et al. Better-than-Average Effect , 2020, Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences.
[36] H. Simon,et al. Rational choice and the structure of the environment. , 1956, Psychological review.
[37] Gordon D. A. Brown,et al. Decision by sampling , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.
[38] K. Fiedler. Explaining and simulating judgment biases as an aggregation phenomenon in probabilistic, multiple-cue environments. , 1996 .
[39] Junqi Shi,et al. Economic Inequality Is Linked to Biased Self-Perception , 2011, Psychological science.
[40] Don A. Moore. Not so Above Average After All: When People Believe They are Worse than Average and its Implications for Theories of Bias in Social Comparison , 2007 .
[41] Anders Winman,et al. The naïve intuitive statistician: a naïve sampling model of intuitive confidence intervals. , 2007, Psychological review.
[42] Kerri L. Johnson,et al. Why the Unskilled are Unaware: Further Explorations of (Absent) Self-Insight Among the Incompetent , 2006, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.