Influence of fingertip contact on illusory arm movements.

Recent evidence suggests that reaching movements are more accurate when end point contact occurs, suggesting that fingertip contact contributes to a final estimation of arm position. In the present study we tested two hypotheses: 1). that fingertip contact influences illusions of arm movement produced by muscle vibration and 2). that this influence depends on the a priori context of the stability of the contact surface. Subjects sat with their elbows on a table and eyes closed. They demonstrated the perceived orientation of the left (cue) arm by mirroring the location with the right (report) arm. We manipulated deep proprioceptive cues by vibrating the left biceps brachia, causing illusions of elbow extension, and tested whether these illusions were altered when the fingertip remained in contact with a stable external surface. The context at this point represents a prior assumption that the external contact surface is stable. Midway through the experiment, the context was changed by challenging the prior assumption that the contact surface was stable by demonstrating that it could move. Unbeknownst to the subject, the external contact surface remained stable during data collection throughout the experiment. As expected, without tactile cues, biceps vibration caused illusory elbow extension. Conditions with fingertip contact and biceps vibration in the stable context demonstrated that contact largely eliminated the overestimation of cue arm elbow angle. However, in the context of a possibly unstable (movable) contact surface, the reports of elbow extension returned. Thus a priori notions about the stability context of an external contact surface influence how this tactile cue is integrated with proprioceptive sensory modalities to generate an estimate of arm location in space. These findings support the notion that tactile cues are used to calibrate proprioception against external spatial frameworks.

[1]  D. Burke,et al.  Discharge of human muscle spindle afferents innervating ankle dorsiflexors during target isometric contractions , 1997, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  O. Bock,et al.  Goal-directed arm movements in absence of visual guidance: evidence for amplitude rather than position control , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  B Craske,et al.  Perception of impossible limb positions induced by tendon vibration. , 1977, Science.

[4]  B. Edin Cutaneous afferents provide information about knee joint movements in humans , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  C. Ghez,et al.  Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  R. Granit The functional role of the muscle spindles--facts and hypotheses. , 1975, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  A. Gordon,et al.  Contribution of tactile information to accuracy in pointing movements , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  M. Latash,et al.  Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  D. McCloskey Differences between the senses of movement and position shown by the effects of loading and vibration of muscles in man. , 1973, Brain research.

[11]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  D. M. MacKay,et al.  Source density analysis of scalp potentials during linguistic and non-linguistic processing of visual stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  F. Horak,et al.  Precision contact of the fingertip reduces postural sway of individuals with bilateral vestibular loss , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Patricia Romaiguère,et al.  Antagonist motor responses correlate with kinesthetic illusions induced by tendon vibration , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  F. W. Cody,et al.  Proprioceptive guidance of human voluntary wrist movements studied using muscle vibration. , 1990, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  J. C. Gilhodes,et al.  Perceptual and motor effects of agonist-antagonist muscle vibration in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  D. F. Collins,et al.  Movement illusions evoked by ensemble cutaneous input from the dorsum of the human hand. , 1996, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Proprioceptive Illusions Induced by Muscle Vibration: Contribution by Muscle Spindles to Perception? , 1972, Science.

[19]  B. Edin,et al.  Skin strain patterns provide kinaesthetic information to the human central nervous system. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  M. Flanders,et al.  Errors in kinesthetic transformations for hand apposition , 1994, Neuroreport.

[21]  Andrew M. Gordon,et al.  Tactile feedback contributes to consistency of finger movements during typing , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  J. Lackner,et al.  Stabilization of posture by precision contact of the index finger. , 1994, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[23]  B. Edin Quantitative analysis of static strain sensitivity in human mechanoreceptors from hairy skin. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  J. Lackner,et al.  Coupling of fingertip somatosensory information to head and body sway , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  J. Lackner,et al.  Fingertip contact suppresses the destabilizing influence of leg muscle vibration. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  J. Lackner Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.