Simultaneous and Successive Contrast Effects in the Perception of Depth from Motion-Parallax and Stereoscopic Information

Prolonged inspection of a three-dimensional corrugated surface resulted in a successive contrast effect, or aftereffect, of depth, whereby a subsequently-viewed physically-flat test surface appeared to be corrugated in depth with the opposite phase to the adapting surface. The aftereffect occurred both when the depth was specified by motion parallax, in the absence of all other sources of depth information, and when it was specified solely by stereoscopic information. The depth aftereffect was measured by ‘nulling’ the apparent depth in the test surface with physical relative motion or binocular disparity until the test surface appeared flat. Up to 70% of the depth in the adapting surface was necessary to null the aftereffect. Simultaneous contrast effects in the perception of three-dimensional surfaces were used to investigate the spatial interactions that exist in the processing of motion-parallax and stereoscopic information. A physically vertical surface appeared to slope in depth in the opposite direction to the slope of a surrounding surface. In this case up to 50% of the slope of the inducing surface was necessary to null the contrast effect. Similar results were again obtained for motion-parallax and stereoscopic depth.

[1]  K. Duncker,et al.  Über induzierte Bewegung , 1929 .

[2]  J. Gibson,et al.  Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines. , 1933 .

[3]  A. Ames Aniseikonia—A Factor in the Functioning of Vision , 1935 .

[4]  H. Wallach,et al.  Figural aftereffects; an investigation of visual processes. , 1944 .

[5]  A. Ames The Space Eikonometer Test for Aniseikonia , 1945 .

[6]  W. Kohler,et al.  Figural after-effects in the third dimensions of visual space. , 1947, The American journal of psychology.

[7]  S. Fernberger A figural after-effect in the third dimension of visual space. , 1948, The American journal of psychology.

[8]  J. Gibson,et al.  The Negative After-Effect of the Perception of a Surface Slanted in the Third Dimension , 1959 .

[9]  N. Sutherland Figural After-Effects and Apparent Size , 1961 .

[10]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Aftereffect of Seen Motion with a Stabilized Retinal Image , 1963, Science.

[11]  I. Howard,et al.  The Effect of Steady Fixation on the Judgement of Relative Depth , 1964 .

[12]  J. Lederberg,et al.  Size Adaptation : A New Aftereffect , 1969 .

[13]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The perceived spatial frequency shift: evidence for frequency‐selective neurones in the human brain , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  P. Wenderoth A Visual Spatial Aftereffect of Surface Slant , 1970 .

[15]  W. Richards,et al.  Anomalous stereoscopic depth perception. , 1971, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[16]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Stereoscopic Depth Aftereffect Produced without Monocular Cues , 1971, Science.

[17]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Evidence for disparity detecting neurones in the human visual system , 1972, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  N Long,et al.  Stereoscopic depth aftereffects with random-dot patterns. , 1973, Vision research.

[19]  K. Nakayama,et al.  A Velocity Analogue of Brightness Contrast , 1973, Perception.

[20]  D. Mackay,et al.  Lateral Interaction between Neural Channels sensitive to Texture Density? , 1973, Nature.

[21]  D E Mitchell,et al.  Stereoscopic aftereffects: evidence for disparity-specific neurones in the human visual system. , 1973, Vision research.

[22]  S. Klein,et al.  The simultaneous spatial frequency shift: a dissociation between the detection and perception of gratings. , 1974, Vision research.

[23]  D. Mitchell,et al.  The spatial selectivity of the tilt aftereffect. , 1974, Vision research.

[24]  C. WILLIAM TYLER,et al.  Depth perception in disparity gratings , 1974, Nature.

[25]  C. Tyler Stereoscopic Tilt and Size Aftereffects , 1975 .

[26]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Simultaneous motion contrast: Velocity, sensitivity and depth response , 1975, Vision Research.

[27]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Chapter 9 – What Does Visual Perception Tell Us About Visual Coding? , 1975 .

[28]  M C Corballis,et al.  Negative aftereffects in visual perception. , 1976, Scientific American.

[29]  Jan J. Koenderink,et al.  Local structure of movement parallax of the plane , 1976 .

[30]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Adaptation alters perceived direction of motion , 1976, Vision Research.

[31]  D Marr,et al.  Early processing of visual information. , 1976, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[32]  S. Anstis,et al.  Interactions between motion aftereffects and induced movement , 1976, Vision Research.

[33]  G. Poggio,et al.  Binocular interaction and depth sensitivity in striate and prestriate cortex of behaving rhesus monkey. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  A Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion for visual depth , 1978, Vision Research.

[35]  R. Schumer,et al.  Independent stereoscopic channels for different extents of spatial pooling , 1979, Vision Research.

[36]  B Rogers,et al.  Motion Parallax as an Independent Cue for Depth Perception , 1979, Perception.

[37]  H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al.  The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[38]  B Moulden,et al.  A Simultaneous Shift in Apparent Direction: Further Evidence for a “Distribution-Shift” Model of Direction Coding , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[39]  B. Rogers,et al.  Similarities between motion parallax and stereopsis in human depth perception , 1982, Vision Research.

[40]  Stereoscopic Depth Aftereffect Produced without Monocular Cues , .