The impact of map orientation and generalisation on congestion decisions: a comparison of schematic-egocentric and topographic-allocentric maps

Map information for drivers is usually presented in an allocentric-topographic form (as with printed maps) or in an egocentric-schematic form (as with road signs). The advent of new variable message boards on UK motorways raises the possibility of presenting road maps to reflect congestion ahead. Should these maps be allocentric-topographic or egocentric-schematic? This was assessed in an eye tracking study, with participants viewing maps of a motorway network in order to identify whether any congestion was relevant to their intended route. The schematic-egocentric maps were responded to most accurately with shorter fixation durations suggesting easier processing. In particular, the driver's entrance and intended exit from the map were attended to more in the allocentric maps. Individual differences in mental rotation ability also seem to contribute to poor performance on allocentric maps. The results favour schematic-egocentric maps for roadside congestion information, but also provide theoretical insights into map-rotation and individual differences. Statement of Relevance: This study informs designers and policy makers about optimum representations of traffic congestion on roadside variable message signs and, furthermore, demonstrates that individual differences contribute to problems with processing certain sign types. Schematic-egocentric representations of a motorway network produced the best results, as noted in behavioural and eye movement measures.

[1]  Alan H S Chan,et al.  Effects of sign characteristics and training methods on safety sign training effectiveness , 2010, Ergonomics.

[2]  A H Wertheim,et al.  Visual conspicuity: A new simple standard, its reliability, validity and applicability , 2010, Ergonomics.

[3]  Katja Kircher,et al.  Effects of road surface appearance and low friction warning systems on driver behaviour and confidence in the warning system , 2009, Ergonomics.

[4]  P Jolicoeur,et al.  Manipulating the shape of distance effects in visual curve tracing: further evidence for the zoom lens model. , 1994, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[5]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[6]  Barry A. T. Brown,et al.  Rotating maps and readers: praxiological aspects of alignment and orientation , 2008 .

[7]  Christoph Hölscher,et al.  How Much Information Do You Need? Schematic Maps in Wayfinding and Self Localisation , 2006, Spatial Cognition.

[8]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[9]  D. Levi,et al.  The effect of similarity and duration on spatial interaction in peripheral vision. , 1994, Spatial vision.

[10]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  Capturing visual attention and the curve tracing operation , 1992 .

[11]  J. Money,et al.  A standardized road-map test of direction sense , 1965 .

[12]  Laura A. Carlson,et al.  Introduction to the special section on spatial reference frames: examining what and how information is encoded through the integration of cognitive, behavioral, and neuroscience approaches. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  Yohtaro Takano,et al.  Mental Rotation , 2008 .

[14]  H. Spekreijse,et al.  The spatial profile of visual attention in mental curve tracing , 2001, Vision Research.

[15]  Yuanzhen Li,et al.  Measuring visual clutter. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[16]  Guy Vingerhoets,et al.  Analysis of the Money Road-Map Test performance in normal and brain-damaged subjects , 1996 .

[17]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Björn N. S. Vlaskamp,et al.  Crowding degrades saccadic search performance , 2005, Vision Research.

[19]  D B Kaber,et al.  Situation awareness and driving performance in a simulated navigation task , 2007, Ergonomics.

[20]  Myriam Chanceaux,et al.  The influence of clutter on real-world scene search: evidence from search efficiency and eye movements. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[21]  Jason S McCarley,et al.  Effects of speed–accuracy instructions on oculomotor scanning and target recognition in a simulated baggage X-ray screening task , 2009, Ergonomics.

[22]  Ovidiu V. Lungu,et al.  Frames of reference during implicit and explicit learning , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Pierre Jolicoeur,et al.  Predicting the shape of distance functions in curve tracing: Evidence for a zoom lens operator , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[24]  S Ullman,et al.  Visual curve tracing properties. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Jonathan W. Kelly,et al.  Multiple systems of spatial memory: evidence from described scenes. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  H. Spekreijse,et al.  A gradual spread of attention during mental curve tracing. , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[28]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Mental space travel: damage to posterior parietal cortex prevents egocentric navigation and reexperiencing of remote spatial memories. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  David Crundall,et al.  Attentional and automatic processes in line tracing: Is tracing obligatory? , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  P. Blignaut Fixation identification: The optimum threshold for a dispersion algorithm , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[31]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Neural correlates , 2023 .

[32]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[33]  E. Gordon,et al.  Defining the temporal threshold for ocular fixation in free-viewing visuocognitive tasks , 2003, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[34]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[35]  C Collet,et al.  Phoning while driving I: a review of epidemiological, psychological, behavioural and physiological studies , 2010, Ergonomics.

[36]  J. O’Keefe Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[37]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Neural correlates of individual differences in spatial learning strategies. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[38]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Hippocampal function and spatial memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging in healthy participants and performance of patients with medial temporal lobe resections. , 2004, Neuropsychology.