Now You'll Feel It, Now You Won't: EEG Rhythms Predict the Effectiveness of Perceptual Masking

Do ongoing brain states determine conscious perception of an upcoming stimulus? Using the high temporal resolution of EEG, we investigated the relationship between prestimulus neuronal oscillations and the perceptibility of two competing somatosensory stimuli embedded in a backward masking paradigm. We identified two prestimulus EEG signatures predictive for a suprathreshold yet weak target stimulus to become perceptually resistant against masking by a stronger distractor stimulus: (i) over left frontal cortex a desynchronization of the regional beta rhythm (∼20 Hz) 500 msec prior to a perceived target, and (ii) a subsequent additional attenuation of both mu (∼10 Hz) and beta “idling” rhythms over those pericentral sensorimotor cortices which are going to process the upcoming target stimulus. Furthermore, across subjects the probability for target perception strongly correlates with the individual absolute level of pre-target amplitudes in these frequency bands and locations. These signatures significantly differed from the EEG characteristics preceding detected and undetected single stimuli. We suggest that the early activation of left frontal areas involved in top–down attentional control is critical for preventing backward masking and leads the preparation of primary sensory cortices: The ensuing prestimulus suppression of sensory idling rhythms warrants an intensified poststimulus processing, and thus, effectively promotes conscious perception of suprathreshold target stimuli embedded into an ecologically relevant condition featuring competing environmental stimuli.

[1]  Stephen J. Anderson,et al.  Elsevier Editorial System(tm) for Brain Research Manuscript Draft Response Letter Reviewer Number 1 Attentional Changes in Pre-stimulus Oscillatory Activity within Early Visual Cortex Are Predictive of Human Visual Performance , 2007 .

[2]  Dominique L. Pritchett,et al.  Neural Correlates of Tactile Detection: A Combined Magnetoencephalography and Biophysically Based Computational Modeling Study , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Prestimulus Oscillatory Activity in the Alpha Band Predicts Visual Discrimination Ability , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  P. Michie,et al.  The effects of spatial selective attention on the somatosensory event-related potential. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[6]  J. Marco-Pallarés,et al.  Location of brain rhythms and their modulation by preparatory attention estimated by current density , 2006, Brain Research.

[7]  E. Miller,et al.  Response to Comment on "Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices" , 2007, Science.

[8]  Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen,et al.  Somatosensory evoked magnetic fields: relation to pre-stimulus mu rhythm , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[9]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[10]  Srikantan S Nagarajan,et al.  Spatiotemporal integration of tactile information in human somatosensory cortex , 2007, BMC Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Boly,et al.  Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Steven Lemm,et al.  Now you feel it--now you don't: ERP correlates of somatosensory awareness. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[13]  Gabriel Curio,et al.  Novel applications of BCI technology: Psychophysiological optimization of working conditions in industry , 2010, 2010 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics.

[14]  T. Ergenoğlu,et al.  Alpha rhythm of the EEG modulates visual detection performance in humans. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  G. Pfurtscheller Central beta rhythm during sensorimotor activities in man. , 1981, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[17]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[18]  C. Shagass,et al.  Early cognitive components of somatosensory event-related potentials. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[19]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  Prestimulus oscillations predict visual perception performance between and within subjects , 2007, NeuroImage.

[20]  C. Brunia,et al.  Anticipatory attention: an event-related desynchronization approach. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[21]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Anterior and posterior attentional control systems use different spatial reference frames: ERP evidence from covert tactile-spatial orienting. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[22]  R. Hari,et al.  Spatiotemporal characteristics of sensorimotor neuromagnetic rhythms related to thumb movement , 1994, Neuroscience.

[23]  Thomas J. Ross,et al.  Neuroanatomical dissociation between bottom–up and top–down processes of visuospatial selective attention , 2006, NeuroImage.

[24]  M. Eimer,et al.  Modulations of early somatosensory ERP components by transient and sustained spatial attention , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  W. Singer,et al.  Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  Jon Driver,et al.  Neural correlates of crossmodal visual-tactile extinction and of tactile awareness revealed by fMRI in a right-hemisphere stroke patient , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[28]  C. Gerloff,et al.  Enhancing cognitive performance with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at human individual alpha frequency , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework , 2001, Cognition.

[30]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Spontaneous fluctuations in posterior alpha-band EEG activity reflect variability in excitability of human visual areas. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  K. Meador,et al.  Physiology of somatosensory perception , 1998, Neurology.

[32]  A. Grinvald,et al.  Dynamics of Ongoing Activity: Explanation of the Large Variability in Evoked Cortical Responses , 1996, Science.

[33]  Martin Eimer,et al.  The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes , 2004, Biological Psychology.

[34]  R. Llinás,et al.  The functional states of the thalamus and the associated neuronal interplay. , 1988, Physiological reviews.

[35]  P. Goldman-Rakic Circuitry of Primate Prefrontal Cortex and Regulation of Behavior by Representational Memory , 2011 .

[36]  Gregor Thut,et al.  Resting electroencephalogram alpha-power over posterior sites indexes baseline visual cortex excitability , 2008, Neuroreport.

[37]  R. Desimone,et al.  Modulation of Oscillatory Neuronal Synchronization by Selective Visual Attention , 2001, Science.

[38]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal correlates of subjective sensory experience , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Jessica J. Green,et al.  Electrical Neuroimaging Reveals Timing of Attentional Control Activity in Human Brain , 2008, PLoS Biology.

[40]  P. Rossini,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus alpha rhythms are related to conscious visual perception: a high-resolution EEG study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  V. Jousmäki,et al.  Modulation of Human Cortical Rolandic Rhythms during Natural Sensorimotor Tasks , 1997, NeuroImage.

[42]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  Population activity in the human dorsal pathway predicts the accuracy of visual motion detection. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  J R Smith,et al.  Physiology of perception , 1999, Neurology.

[44]  Matti S. Hämäläinen,et al.  Transformations in oscillatory activity and evoked responses in primary somatosensory cortex in middle age: A combined computational neural modeling and MEG study , 2010, NeuroImage.

[45]  C Tomberg,et al.  Mapping early somatosensory evoked potentials in selective attention: critical evaluation of control conditions used for titrating by difference the cognitive P30, P40, P100 and N140. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[46]  M Hämäläinen,et al.  Early deflections of cerebral magnetic responses to median nerve stimulation. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[47]  Jon Driver,et al.  The electrophysiology of tactile extinction: ERP correlates of unconscious somatosensory processing , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  M. Brandt,et al.  The relationship between prestimulus-alpha amplitude and visual evoked potential amplitude. , 1991, The International journal of neuroscience.

[49]  K. Linkenkaer-Hansen,et al.  Prestimulus Oscillations Enhance Psychophysical Performance in Humans , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[51]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  A shift of visual spatial attention is selectively associated with human EEG alpha activity , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[53]  B. Libet,et al.  Responses of Human Somatosensory Cortex to Stimuli below Threshold for Conscious Sensation , 1967, Science.

[54]  Kimron Shapiro,et al.  Modulation of long-range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations of visual attention in humans. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[55]  K. Linkenkaer-Hansen,et al.  Early Neural Correlates of Conscious Somatosensory Perception , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Vernon B Mountcastle,et al.  Brain Mechanisms for Directed Attention 1 , 1978, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[57]  W. Klimesch,et al.  EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis , 2007, Brain Research Reviews.

[58]  E. Basar,et al.  Enhancement of visual evoked potentials by stimulation during low prestimulus EEG stages. , 1993, The International journal of neuroscience.