Hand-held tools with complex kinematics are efficiently incorporated into movement planning and online control.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Atsushi Iriki,et al. Shaping multisensory action–space with tools: evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[2] J. R. Simon. The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .
[3] B. Hommel. Inverting the Simon effect by intention , 1993 .
[4] H. Sakata,et al. Parietal control of hand action , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[5] Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al. Short-term plasticity of the primary somatosensory cortex during tool use , 2004, Neuroreport.
[6] M. Jeannerod,et al. Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 1991, Experimental Brain Research.
[7] R. Douglas,et al. Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[8] W. Epstein,et al. Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] W A MacKay,et al. Properties of reach-related neuronal activity in cortical area 7A. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.
[10] J R Flanagan,et al. Trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation: evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] L. G. Gawryszewski,et al. What is crossed in crossed-hand effects? , 1986 .
[12] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[13] G. Stratton. Some preliminary experiments on vision without inversion of the retinal image. , 1896 .
[14] M Hallett,et al. Time course of determination of movement direction in the reaction time task in humans. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] Hermann von Helmholtz,et al. Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1962 .
[16] M. Desmurget,et al. On-line motor control in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[17] T. Schormann,et al. Activation in the Ipsilateral Posterior Parietal Cortex during Tool Use: A PET Study , 2001, NeuroImage.
[18] Herbert Heuer,et al. Enhanced mechanical transparency during practice impedes open-loop control of a complex tool , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] S. Kinomura,et al. A PET Study of Visuomotor Learning under Optical Rotation , 2000, NeuroImage.
[20] M. Goodale,et al. Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] J F Kalaska,et al. Rapid online correction is selectively suppressed during movement with a visuomotor transformation. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] M. Tanaka,et al. Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.
[23] W. Medendorp,et al. Behavioral and cortical mechanisms for spatial coding and action planning , 2008, Cortex.
[24] Matthew Heath,et al. Visuomotor mental rotation: Reaction time is not a function of the angle of rotation , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.
[25] H. Cunningham. Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] P. Brown,et al. Evidence for subcortical involvement in the visual control of human reaching. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[27] G. Holmes,et al. Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions , 1911 .
[28] Bijan Pesaran,et al. Reaction Time Correlations during Eye–Hand Coordination: Behavior and Modeling , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[29] Bruce Bridgeman,et al. Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.
[30] M. Desmurget,et al. An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[31] B. Day,et al. Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[32] J. Randall Flanagan,et al. Representations of Tool Dynamics Mediate Skillful Manipulation , 2010 .
[33] J F Kalaska,et al. Integration of predictive feedforward and sensory feedback signals for online control of visually guided movement. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[34] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[35] M. Goodale,et al. Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 1986, Experimental Brain Research.
[36] M. Husain,et al. The Role of the Posterior Parietal Lobe in Prism Adaptation: Failure to Adapt to Optical Prisms in a Patient with Bilateral Damage to Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2006, Cortex.
[37] Tutis Vilis,et al. Human parietal "reach region" primarily encodes intrinsic visual direction, not extrinsic movement direction, in a visual motor dissociation task. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[38] H. Onoe,et al. Functional Brain Mapping of Monkey Tool Use , 2001, NeuroImage.
[39] J. Flanagan,et al. Relation between reaction time and reach errors during visuomotor adaptation , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.
[40] Juan Fernandez-Ruiz,et al. Rapid Topographical Plasticity of the Visuomotor Spatial Transformation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[41] A. P. Georgopoulos,et al. Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.
[42] C. Prablanc,et al. Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.
[43] A. Berti,et al. When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[44] Richard A Andersen,et al. Forward estimation of movement state in posterior parietal cortex , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[45] Y. Guiard,et al. The lateral coding of rotations: a study of the simon effect with wheel-rotation responses. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.
[46] C. Prablanc,et al. Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.
[47] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[48] Heike Weber,et al. Effects of stimulus conditions on the performance of antisaccades in man , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[49] P. Fitts,et al. S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[50] S. Ichinose,et al. Extension of Corticocortical Afferents into the Anterior Bank of the Intraparietal Sulcus by Tool-use Training in Adult Monkeys , 2005 .
[51] G. M. Redding,et al. Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[52] A. Maravita,et al. Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[53] J. Vercher,et al. Target and hand position information in the online control of goal-directed arm movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.