Hand-held tools with complex kinematics are efficiently incorporated into movement planning and online control.

Certain hand-held tools alter the mapping between hand motion and motion of the tool end point that must be controlled in order to perform a task. For example, when using a pool cue, the motion of the cue tip is reversed relative to the hand. Previous studies have shown that the time required to initiate a reaching movement (Fernandez-Ruiz J, Wong W, Armstrong IT, Flanagan JR. Behav Brain Res 219: 8-14, 2011), or correct an ongoing reaching movement (Gritsenko V, Kalaska JF. J Neurophysiol 104: 3084-3104, 2010), is prolonged when the mapping between hand motion and motion of a cursor controlled by the hand is reversed. Here we show that these time costs can be significantly reduced when the reversal is instantiated by a virtual hand-held tool. Participants grasped the near end of a virtual tool, consisting of a rod connecting two circles, and moved the end point to displayed targets. In the reversal condition, the rod translated through, and rotated about, a pivot point such that there was a left-right reversal between hand and end point motion. In the nonreversal control, the tool translated with the hand. As expected, when only the two circles were presented, movement initiation and correction times were much longer in the reversal condition. However, when full vision of the tool was provided, the reaction time cost was almost eliminated. These results indicate that tools with complex kinematics can be efficiently incorporated into sensorimotor control mechanisms used in movement planning and online control.

[1]  Atsushi Iriki,et al.  Shaping multisensory action–space with tools: evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  J. R. Simon The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .

[3]  B. Hommel Inverting the Simon effect by intention , 1993 .

[4]  H. Sakata,et al.  Parietal control of hand action , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[5]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Short-term plasticity of the primary somatosensory cortex during tool use , 2004, Neuroreport.

[6]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 1991, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  R. Douglas,et al.  Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  W. Epstein,et al.  Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  W A MacKay,et al.  Properties of reach-related neuronal activity in cortical area 7A. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  J R Flanagan,et al.  Trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation: evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  L. G. Gawryszewski,et al.  What is crossed in crossed-hand effects? , 1986 .

[12]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[13]  G. Stratton Some preliminary experiments on vision without inversion of the retinal image. , 1896 .

[14]  M Hallett,et al.  Time course of determination of movement direction in the reaction time task in humans. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Hermann von Helmholtz,et al.  Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1962 .

[16]  M. Desmurget,et al.  On-line motor control in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  T. Schormann,et al.  Activation in the Ipsilateral Posterior Parietal Cortex during Tool Use: A PET Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[18]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Enhanced mechanical transparency during practice impedes open-loop control of a complex tool , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  S. Kinomura,et al.  A PET Study of Visuomotor Learning under Optical Rotation , 2000, NeuroImage.

[20]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  J F Kalaska,et al.  Rapid online correction is selectively suppressed during movement with a visuomotor transformation. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[23]  W. Medendorp,et al.  Behavioral and cortical mechanisms for spatial coding and action planning , 2008, Cortex.

[24]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Visuomotor mental rotation: Reaction time is not a function of the angle of rotation , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[25]  H. Cunningham Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  P. Brown,et al.  Evidence for subcortical involvement in the visual control of human reaching. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  G. Holmes,et al.  Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions , 1911 .

[28]  Bijan Pesaran,et al.  Reaction Time Correlations during Eye–Hand Coordination: Behavior and Modeling , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[30]  M. Desmurget,et al.  An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  B. Day,et al.  Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  J. Randall Flanagan,et al.  Representations of Tool Dynamics Mediate Skillful Manipulation , 2010 .

[33]  J F Kalaska,et al.  Integration of predictive feedforward and sensory feedback signals for online control of visually guided movement. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 1986, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  M. Husain,et al.  The Role of the Posterior Parietal Lobe in Prism Adaptation: Failure to Adapt to Optical Prisms in a Patient with Bilateral Damage to Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2006, Cortex.

[37]  Tutis Vilis,et al.  Human parietal "reach region" primarily encodes intrinsic visual direction, not extrinsic movement direction, in a visual motor dissociation task. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  H. Onoe,et al.  Functional Brain Mapping of Monkey Tool Use , 2001, NeuroImage.

[39]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Relation between reaction time and reach errors during visuomotor adaptation , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[40]  Juan Fernandez-Ruiz,et al.  Rapid Topographical Plasticity of the Visuomotor Spatial Transformation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  A. Berti,et al.  When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  Richard A Andersen,et al.  Forward estimation of movement state in posterior parietal cortex , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  Y. Guiard,et al.  The lateral coding of rotations: a study of the simon effect with wheel-rotation responses. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[46]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[47]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Heike Weber,et al.  Effects of stimulus conditions on the performance of antisaccades in man , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  P. Fitts,et al.  S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[50]  S. Ichinose,et al.  Extension of Corticocortical Afferents into the Anterior Bank of the Intraparietal Sulcus by Tool-use Training in Adult Monkeys , 2005 .

[51]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  A. Maravita,et al.  Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[53]  J. Vercher,et al.  Target and hand position information in the online control of goal-directed arm movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.