The frequency of saccades correlates to peak velocity in symmetrical disparity vergence

A pure vergence stimulus requires the two eyes to turn equally inward or outward theoretically resulting in a pure symmetrical vergence response. However, saccades, a rapid conjugate eye movement, are frequently observed in vergence responses. This investigation sought to systematically quantify whether the occurrence of saccades within symmetrical vergence responses is correlated to vergence peak velocity. Eye movements are quantified using a limbus tracking system from three subjects. Symmetrical convergence and divergence 4° step responses with an initial position located at far or near which are known to evoke different peak velocities are analyzed. Data are quantified via peak velocity. A saccade detecting algorithm is utilized to quantify the frequency of saccades in the transient portion (first second) of vergence responses. Near convergence responses are slower than far convergence and far divergence responses are slower compared to near divergence movements. The occurrence of saccades is negatively correlated to vergence peak velocity. When the velocity is slower, the number of saccades increases. This study suggests that the brain may initiate a saccade to facilitate a slow vergence movement, potentially to allow object recognition before binocular fusion.

[1]  Zoï Kapoula,et al.  Saccades during symmetrical vergence , 2008, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.

[2]  Tara L. Alvarez,et al.  Sustained Fixation Induced Changes in Phoria and Convergence Peak Velocity , 2011, PloS one.

[3]  N. Chiaravalloti,et al.  Vision Therapy in Adults with Convergence Insufficiency: Clinical and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Measures , 2010, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[4]  Tara L Alvarez,et al.  The relationship between phoria and the ratio of convergence peak velocity to divergence peak velocity. , 2010, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[5]  L M Optican,et al.  Saccade-vergence interactions in humans. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  J. Semmlow,et al.  Saccadic Behavior during the Response to Pure Disparity Vergence Stimuli I : General Properties , 2008 .

[7]  J. Semmlow,et al.  Initial component control in disparity vergence: a model-based study , 1998, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[8]  M. T. Bergen,et al.  Short-term predictive changes in the dynamics of disparity vergence eye movements. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[9]  J. E. Lieberman,et al.  Frequency limitations of the two-point central difference differentiation algorithm , 1982, Biological Cybernetics.

[10]  Casper J. Erkelens,et al.  Voluntary binocular gaze-shifts in the plane of regard: Dynamics of version and vergence , 1995, Vision Research.

[11]  J. Semmlow,et al.  Saccadic Behavior during the Response to Pure Vergence Stimuli I: General Properties , 2007 .

[12]  J. Semmlow,et al.  Correction of Saccade-Induced Midline Errors in Responses to Pure Disparity Vergence Stimuli. , 2009 .

[13]  Haluk Ögmen,et al.  Vergence Dynamics Predict Fixation Disparity , 2001, Neural Computation.

[14]  H. Ogmen,et al.  Neural network model of short-term horizontal disparity vergence dynamics. , 1997, Vision research.

[15]  Chung Chang,et al.  Sustained convergence induced changes in phoria and divergence dynamics , 2009, Vision Research.