Mapping as a Cultural Universal

This chapter discusses the hypothesis that mapping behavior, the making of map like models, is a cultural universal, an important component of ecological behavior. The essay presents a theoretical framework for the hypothesis and discusses three categories of evidence developmental, prehistoric, and cross-cultural — which support the hypothesis. Humans must visualize, analyze, describe, and communicate the nature of large environments perceived atomistically, and therefore they create material representations depicting environments as if seen as a whole, from overhead. The result is an organized sign system with certain linguistic properties, including two syntactic transformations (rotation/projection and scale reduction), and the semantic representation of landscape features as iconic or abstract signs. This concept of the map yields useful criteria for the identification and study of maps in culture, history, and behavior. Many examples of prehistoric imagery, mostly parietal, extending to periods earlier than the Neolithic (of both geographical hemispheres), appear map-like, giving evidence of rotated, scale-reduced, and abstracted depiction of the environment and suggesting that mapping may have represented a form of adaptive behavior for modern humans. In a few cases, which are discussed, the representation depicts a real local landscape. Ethnographic studies, while in general not concerned with mapping, have provided evidence that mapping activity occurs in many contemporary cultures. Studies of the behavior of very young children, finally, indicate that mapping abilities appear much earlier than generally supposed, and seem to play an important role in early development.1

[1]  William Davenport Marshall islands navigational charts , 1960 .

[2]  A. Platt Toxic Green: The Trouble with Golf. , 1994 .

[3]  E. Spelke Origins of visual knowledge , 1990 .

[4]  N. Munn,et al.  Walbiri Iconography: Graphic Representation and Cultural Symbolism in a Central Australian Society , 1986 .

[5]  E. Brunswik,et al.  The Conceptual Framework of Psychology , 1954 .

[6]  P. Ucko Form in Indigenous Art Schematisation in the Art of Aboriginal Australia and Prehistoric Europe , 1977 .

[7]  R. Downs,et al.  Image and Environment: Cognitive Mapping and Spatial Behavior , 2017 .

[8]  David Woodward,et al.  The history of cartography , 1989 .

[9]  R. Carnap,et al.  INTERNATIONAL ENCYCLOPEDIA OF UNIFIED SCIENCE. , 1939, Science.

[10]  M. H. Matthews Young children's representations of the environment: A comparison of techniques , 1985 .

[11]  L. Acredolo,et al.  Developmental Changes in Map-Reading Skills. , 1979 .

[12]  The Colonizer's Model of the World: Geographical Diffusionism and Eurocentric History. , 1996 .

[13]  A. Betts,et al.  Rock Art in Eastern Jordan : 'Kite' Carvings ? , 1986 .

[14]  C Grant Head,et al.  The Map As Natural Language: A Paradigm For Understanding , 1984 .

[15]  M. Blades,et al.  The Child in the Physical Environment: The Development of Spatial Knowledge and Cognition , 1989 .

[16]  R. Gould Spears and spearthrowers of the Western Desert aborigines of Australia , 1970 .

[17]  James M. Blaut,et al.  The Colonizer's Model of the World: Geographical Diffusionism and Eurocentric History. , 1993 .

[18]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Thought Without Language , 1988 .

[19]  Noam Chomsky Language and Problems of Knowledge , 1987 .

[20]  Ervin H. Zube,et al.  Advance in Environment, Behavior, and Design , 1989, Advances in Environment, Behavior, and Design.

[21]  M. H. Matthews Environmental Cognition of Young Children: Images of Journey to School and Home Area , 1984 .

[22]  T. Grieder Periods in Pecos Style Pictographs , 1966, American Antiquity.

[23]  M. G. McGee,et al.  Human spatial abilities: Sources of sex differences , 1979 .

[24]  Mapping at the Age of Three , 1974 .

[25]  R. Hart,et al.  Children's experience of place , 1979 .

[26]  Michael Blakemore,et al.  From Way-Finding to Map-Making: The Spatial Information Fields of Aboriginal Peoples , 1981 .

[27]  A. A. Lyutyy ON THE ESSENCE OF THE LANGUAGE OF THE MAP , 1986 .

[28]  Christopher Spencer,et al.  The development of iconic mapping ability in young children , 1980 .

[29]  Hansgeorg Schlichtmann Characteristic Traits of the Semiotic System 'Map Symbolism' , 1985 .

[30]  Clark C. Presson The development of map-reading skills. , 1982, Child development.

[31]  David Stea,et al.  Participatory and Action Research Methods , 1991 .

[32]  W. Massey Anthropological papers of the University of Arizona , 1960 .

[33]  Jeffrey C. Patton,et al.  Notes Toward A Theory Of Mapping Behavior , 1989 .

[34]  Margaret Schadler,et al.  The Development of Young Children's Spatial Representations of Their Classrooms. , 1977 .

[35]  Robert Lloyd,et al.  Cognitive Maps: Encoding and Decoding Information , 1989 .

[36]  H. Wallace,et al.  Petroglyphs of the Picacho Mountains, South Central Arizona , 1986 .

[37]  J. M. Blaut Environmental Mapping in Young Children , 1970 .

[38]  J. Piaget,et al.  Psychology and epistemology , 1971 .

[39]  Sharon Pray Muir Understanding and Improving Students' Map Reading Skills , 1985, The Elementary School Journal.

[40]  M. Posner Foundations of cognitive science , 1989 .

[41]  Henry M. Wellman,et al.  Children's Searching : The Development of Search Skill and Spatial Representation , 1985 .

[42]  J. Gunn,et al.  Upper Paleolithic Symbol Systems of the Russian Plain: Cognitive and Comparative Analysis [and Comments and Reply] , 1979, Current Anthropology.

[43]  F. J. Langdon,et al.  The Child's Conception of Space , 1967 .

[44]  R. Gould Spears and spear-throwers of the Western Desert aborigines of Australia. American Museum novitates ; no. 2403 , 1970 .

[45]  R. Byrne,et al.  Pointing to preschool children's spatial competence: A study in natural settings , 1984 .

[46]  D. Stea,et al.  Studies of Geographic Learning. , 1971 .

[47]  L E Berk,et al.  Why children talk to themselves. , 1985, Scientific American.

[48]  M. Wolfenstein Children's Humor: A Psychological Analysis , 1978 .

[49]  J. Hollerbach,et al.  Visual cognition and action , 1990 .

[50]  W. Doolittle Pre-Hispanic Occupance in the Valley of Sonora, Mexico: Archaeological Confirmations of Early Spanish Reports , 1990 .

[51]  M. Blades,et al.  The use of maps by 4–6‐year‐old children in a large‐scale maze , 1987 .

[52]  Torgny Ottosson What does it take to read a map , 1988 .

[53]  M. Blades,et al.  The development of children's ability to use spatial representations. , 1994, Advances in child development and behavior.

[54]  Harold M. Proshansky,et al.  The Development of Place Identity in the Child , 1987 .

[55]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Spatial representation and behavior across the life span , 1981 .

[56]  M. Monmonier How to Lie with Maps , 1991 .

[57]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Early map use as an unlearned ability , 1986, Cognition.

[58]  F. J. Langdon,et al.  The Child's Conception of Space , 1967 .

[59]  Ron Wallace Cognitive Mapping and the Origin of Language and Mind , 1989, Current Anthropology.

[60]  David V. Canter,et al.  Psychology and the built environment , 1976 .

[61]  T. Abel,et al.  Mind, Self, and Society , 1934 .

[62]  J. Bruner Actual minds, possible worlds , 1985 .

[63]  W. H. Ittelson Environment and cognition , 1974 .