Aging and filtering by movement in visual search.

We examined the ability of younger and older adults to selectively process moving items and ignore stationary items in a task that required the search for a target defined by a conjunction of movement and form (i.e., search for a moving X among moving Os and stationary Xs) in displays of 5, 9, 17, and 25 stimuli (Experiment 1) and displays of 5, 10, and 20 stimuli (Experiment 2). We also investigated subjects' performance in two feature search tasks, the search for a target defined by movement or form. Finally, we examined the influence of practice on feature and conjunction search. Younger and older adults searched the displays at similar rates in the feature and conjunction search tasks. Older and younger adults also benefited equivalently from practice. These data suggest age-equivalence in the processes which underlie feature search in dynamic environments as well as those processes responsible for the segregation of moving and stationary objects in the visual field.

[1]  J. Colombo,et al.  Visual pop-out in infants: Evidence for preattentive search in 3- and 4-month-olds , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  C. Scialfa,et al.  Age differences in the useful field of view: an eye movement analysis. , 1994, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[3]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[4]  H. Müller,et al.  Perceptual integration of motion and form information: is the movement filter involved in form discrimination? , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  P. D. Spear,et al.  Neural bases of visual deficits during aging , 1993, Vision Research.

[6]  J. Zacks,et al.  Visual search times assessed without reaction times: a new method and an application to aging. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  T. Stuve,et al.  Motion perception and aging. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[8]  J. Duncan,et al.  Beyond the search surface: visual search and attentional engagement. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  T D Albright,et al.  Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. , 1992, Science.

[10]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Automatization and training in visual search. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[11]  A. D. Fisk,et al.  Are age differences in consistent-mapping visual search due to feature learning or attention training? , 1991, Psychology and aging.

[12]  P. McLeod,et al.  Filtering by movement in visual search. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  A. Treisman,et al.  Conjunction search revisited. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Jon Driver,et al.  Selective deficit of visual search in moving displays after extrastriate damage , 1989, Nature.

[16]  D. Plude,et al.  Aging, selective attention, and feature integration. , 1989, Psychology and aging.

[17]  S. Dehaene Discriminability and dimensionality effects invisual search for featural conjunctions: A functional pop-out , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  D. Roenker,et al.  Age and visual search: expanding the useful field of view. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[19]  Jon Driver,et al.  Visual search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel , 1988, Nature.

[20]  A Treisman,et al.  Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.

[21]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Visual search for targets defined by combinations of color, shape, and size: An examination of the task constraints on feature and conjunction searches , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  David J. Madden,et al.  Aging, attention, and the use of meaning during visual search , 1987 .

[24]  S B Steinman,et al.  Serial and Parallel Search in Pattern Vision? , 1987, Perception.

[25]  C. Scialfa,et al.  Age differences in target identification as a function of retinal location and noise level: examination of the useful field of view. , 1987, Psychology and aging.

[26]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Visual localization: age and practice. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[27]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions , 1986, Nature.

[28]  F. Royer,et al.  Aging and similarity grouping in visual search. , 1985, Journal of gerontology.

[29]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Toward a Model of Attention and the Development of Automatic Processing. , 1984 .

[30]  M. Albert,et al.  The effect of increasing age on the latency for saccadic eye movements. , 1983, Journal of gerontology.

[31]  R. Nebes,et al.  The use of focused attention in visual search by young and old adults. , 1983, Experimental aging research.

[32]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  J. Ford,et al.  Event-related potentials recorded from young and old adults during a memory retrieval task. , 1979, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[34]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  AN AGE-DECREMENT IN THE ABILITY TO IGNORE IRRELEVANT INFORMATION. , 1965, Journal of gerontology.

[35]  M. Wertheimer Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt. II , 1923 .

[36]  Max Wertheimer,et al.  Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt , 2017 .