The in-depth cognitive levels of imagination of artisans and designers

This paper investigated the in-depth cognitive levels of artisans (craft artisans) and designers (design trainers) as they observed design problems (in the early stage of idea generation) during design training. A concept network method based on the associative concept dictionary was employed to extract the verbalised thoughts of four artisans and four designers. We then identified semantic relationships based on a factor analysis. We discovered that, in contrast to designers, artisans tended to activate lower in-depth cognitive levels. Our study demonstrated that artisans placed greater focus on product appearance and technical aspects, such as operation (replace, reduce, etc.) and shape (waist, body, etc.). In contrast, designers paid greater attention to the presence of issues related to surroundings, such as scene (silverware, norm, etc.) and appeal (fresh, dish, etc.). These results demonstrate that closely or remotely associated concepts correlate with shallow or in-depth cognitive levels based on creative solutions.

[1]  Christos Faloutsos,et al.  Dynamics of large networks , 2008 .

[2]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data , 1984 .

[3]  Robert P. Smith,et al.  A predictive model of sequential iteration in engineering design , 1997 .

[4]  Liane Gabora,et al.  An Agent-based Simulation of the Effectiveness of Creative Leadership , 2010, ArXiv.

[5]  Yukari Nagai,et al.  A Method to Generate and Evaluate a Creative Design Idea by Focusing on Associative Process , 2009 .

[6]  Thomas A. Schreiber,et al.  The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[7]  A. Mwihaki Meaning and use: a functional view of semantics and pragmatics , 2012 .

[8]  Robert L. Canfield,et al.  The Ecology of Rural Ethnic Groups and the Spatial Dimensions of Power , 1973 .

[9]  W. Maki,et al.  Latent structure in measures of associative, semantic, and thematic knowledge , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  G. A. Davis,et al.  Barriers to Creativity and Creative Attitudes , 2011 .

[11]  Steven M. Smith,et al.  The creative cognition approach. , 1995 .

[12]  C. Martindale Creativity and connectionism. , 1995 .

[13]  Andreas Fink,et al.  Creativity and personality in professional dancers , 2011 .

[14]  A. Mwihaki MEANING AS USE : A FUNCTIONAL VIEW OF SEMANTICS AND PRAGMATICS , 2005 .

[15]  J. Cusack Creative Cognition: Theory, Research, and Applications , 1994 .

[16]  G. Goldschmidt,et al.  How good are good ideas? Correlates of design creativity , 2005 .

[17]  Shulamith Kreitler,et al.  The Cognitive Profile of Creativity in Design. , 2011 .

[18]  Georgi V. Georgiev,et al.  A methodology to analyse in-depth impressions of design on the basis of concept networks , 2011 .

[19]  S. Mednick,et al.  CONTINUAL ASSOCIATION AS A FUNCTION OF LEVEL OF CREATIVITY AND TYPE OF VERBAL STIMULUS. , 1964, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[20]  Remko van der Lugt,et al.  How sketching can affect the idea generation process in design group meetings , 2005 .

[21]  Henri Christiaans,et al.  Creativity in Design Engineering and the Role of Knowledge: Modelling the Expert , 2005 .

[22]  Noah E. Friedkin,et al.  Theoretical Foundations for Centrality Measures , 1991, American Journal of Sociology.

[23]  John Baer Creativity and Divergent Thinking: A Task-Specific Approach , 1993 .

[24]  Yukari Nagai,et al.  Virtual Impression Networks for Capturing Deep Impressions , 2011, DCC.

[25]  Georgi V. Georgiev,et al.  A conceptual network analysis of user impressions and meanings of product materials in design , 2011 .

[26]  N. Cross Designerly ways of knowing , 2006 .