Signalling and the Evolution of Cooperative Foraging in Dynamic Environments
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] T. Valone,et al. Public Information: From Nosy Neighbors to Cultural Evolution , 2004, Science.
[2] Sasha R. X. Dall. Can information sharing explain recruitment to food from communal roosts , 2002 .
[3] P. Ward,et al. THE IMPORTANCE OF CERTAIN ASSEMBLAGES OF BIRDS AS “INFORMATION‐CENTRES” FOR FOOD‐FINDING , 2008 .
[4] L. Dugatkin,et al. On the evolution of delayed recruitment to food bonanzas , 1999 .
[5] D. Floreano,et al. Evolutionary Conditions for the Emergence of Communication in Robots , 2007, Current Biology.
[6] L. Dugatkin. Cooperation Among Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective , 1997 .
[7] Sasha R. X. Dall,et al. Rich Pickings Near Large Communal Roosts Favor ‘Gang’ Foraging by Juvenile Common Ravens, Corvus corax , 2009, PloS one.
[8] Åke Brännström,et al. Synergy in social communication , 2008 .
[9] L. Giraldeau,et al. Breeding colonies as information centers: a reappraisal of information-based hypotheses using the producer-scrounger game , 2001 .
[10] Tim Clutton-Brock,et al. Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies , 2009, Nature.
[11] N. Buckley. Spatial‐Concentration Effects and the Importance of Local Enhancement in the Evolution of Colonial Breeding in Seabirds , 1997, The American Naturalist.
[12] T. Vincent,et al. Evolution of cooperation with shared costs and benefits , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[13] M. Egas,et al. Beyond the information centre hypothesis: communal roosting for information on food, predators, travel companions and mates? , 2010 .
[14] G. Hardin,et al. The Tragedy of the Commons , 1968, Green Planet Blues.
[15] Craig W. Steele,et al. Effects of group size on the responsiveness of zebrafish, Brachydanio rerio (Hamilton Buchanan), to alanine, a chemical attractant , 1991 .
[16] D. Grünbaum. Schooling as a strategy for taxis in a noisy environment , 1998, Evolutionary Ecology.
[17] H. Richner,et al. Is the Information Center Hypothesis a Flop , 1995 .
[18] M. Nowak. Five Rules for the Evolution of Cooperation , 2006, Science.
[19] Colin Torney,et al. Context-dependent interaction leads to emergent search behavior in social aggregates , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[20] R. Johnstone,et al. Animal signals , 2013, Current Biology.
[21] G. Wilkinson. Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat , 1984, Nature.
[22] A. Zahavi. The evolution of communal roosts as information centers and the pitfall of group selection: a rejoinder to Richner and Heeb , 1996 .
[23] Charles R. Brown,et al. Food-sharing signals among socially foraging cliff swallows , 1991, Animal Behaviour.
[24] C. Clark,et al. Foraging and Flocking Strategies: Information in an Uncertain Environment , 1984, The American Naturalist.
[25] S. Levin. Games, groups, and the global good , 2009 .
[26] S. Levin. THE PROBLEM OF PATTERN AND SCALE IN ECOLOGY , 1992 .
[27] P. Marler,et al. Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): I. Socioecological factors , 1993 .
[28] Z. Barta,et al. The role of information transfer under different food patterns: a simulation study , 1992 .
[29] Iain D. Couzin,et al. Specialization and evolutionary branching within migratory populations , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[30] H. Berg. Random Walks in Biology , 2018 .
[31] I. Couzin,et al. Social interactions, information use, and the evolution of collective migration , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[32] H. Gintis,et al. Costly signaling and cooperation. , 2001, Journal of theoretical biology.
[33] B. Heinrich. Ravens in Winter , 1989 .
[34] B. Heinrich,et al. Do common ravens yell because they want to attract others? , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[35] C. Clark,et al. The evolutionary advantages of group foraging , 1986 .
[36] Charles R. Brown,et al. Social foraging in cliff swallows: local enhancement, risk sensitivity, competition and the avoidance of predators , 1988, Animal Behaviour.
[37] Frank W. Grasso,et al. Lévy-taxis: a novel search strategy for finding odor plumes in turbulent flow-dominated environments , 2009 .
[38] S. Levin,et al. Crossing scales, crossing disciplines: collective motion and collective action in the Global Commons† , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[39] R. Michod. Darwinian Dynamics: Evolutionary Transitions in Fitness and Individuality , 1999 .
[40] H. Richner,et al. Communal life: honest signaling and the recruitment center hypothesis , 1996 .
[41] T. Valone. Group foraging, public information, and patch estimation , 1989 .
[42] Frederic Bartumeus,et al. ANIMAL SEARCH STRATEGIES: A QUANTITATIVE RANDOM‐WALK ANALYSIS , 2005 .
[43] J. M. Sancho,et al. Langevin approach to generate synthetic turbulent flows , 1997, chao-dyn/9703015.
[44] W. Hamilton. The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. , 1964, Journal of theoretical biology.
[45] Sasha R. X. Dall,et al. Biological information in an ecological context , 2010 .
[46] David P. Hughes,et al. Sociobiology of communication : an interdisciplinary perspective , 2008 .
[47] S. Levin. The problem of pattern and scale in ecology , 1992 .
[48] W. Hamilton. Geometry for the selfish herd. , 1971, Journal of theoretical biology.
[49] K. Vahala. Handbook of stochastic methods for physics, chemistry and the natural sciences , 1986, IEEE Journal of Quantum Electronics.
[50] Sasha R. X. Dall,et al. Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[51] D. Goldstein. Estimates of daily energy expenditure in birds: the time-energy budget as an integrator of laboratory and field studies , 1988 .
[52] D. Floreano,et al. The evolution of information suppression in communicating robots with conflicting interests , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[53] W. Hamilton. The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. , 1964, Journal of theoretical biology.
[54] D. Queller. Kinship, reciprocity and synergism in the evolution of social behaviour , 1985, Nature.
[55] S. Levin. Patchiness in marine and terrestrial systems: from individuals to populations , 1994 .
[56] Subramanian Ramamoorthy. Games, Groups and the Global Good , 2013, J. Oper. Res. Soc..
[57] M. Elgar. The establishment of foraging flocks in house sparrows: risk of predation and daily temperature , 1986, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[58] R. C. Miller. The Significance of the Gregarious Habit , 1922 .
[59] Marcos,et al. Resource Patch Formation and Exploitation throughout the Marine Microbial Food Web , 2008, The American Naturalist.
[60] Craig W. Benkman,et al. The use and misuse of public information by foraging red crossbills , 1999 .
[61] T. Caraco,et al. Social Foraging Theory , 2018 .
[62] T. Caraco,et al. Co-operative and non-co-operative bases of food-calling , 1989 .
[63] T. Kármán. Progress in the Statistical Theory of Turbulence , 1948 .
[64] J. Ottino. The Kinematics of Mixing: Stretching, Chaos, and Transport , 1989 .