Spatial and velocity tuning of processes underlying induced motion

A nulling procedure was used to quantify the velocity and spatial frequency tuning of induced motion for sinusoidal gratings. For each spatial frequency of test and inducing gratings, there was a range of low velocities which resulted in strong induction, with a gain of close to 1. For low spatial frequencies induction occurred at higher velocities than was the case for high spatial frequencies. Induced motion shows bandpass spatial frequency tuning, with a bandwidth of about two octaves at half-height. Induced motion appears to be mediated by spatial channels with a low pass temporal characteristic. To a first approximation, induced motion appears to be a product of velocity and spatial frequency.

[1]  J. Allik,et al.  Reaction time to motion onset:Local dispersion model analysis , 1984, Vision Research.

[2]  A. Pantle Motion aftereffect magnitude as a measure of the spatio-temporal response properties of direction-sensitive analyzers. , 1974, Vision research.

[3]  D. Burr,et al.  Contrast sensitivity at high velocities , 1982, Vision Research.

[4]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Relative motion induced between stationary lines , 1978, Vision Research.

[5]  A Pantle,et al.  On the Capacity of Directionally Selective Mechanisms to Encode Different Dimensions of Moving Stimuli , 1978, Perception.

[6]  A. Watanabe,et al.  Spatial sine-wave responses of the human visual system. , 1968, Vision research.

[7]  A. Pantle,et al.  Velocity-sensitive elements in human vision: initial psychophysical evidence. , 1968, Vision research.

[8]  B G Breitmeyer,et al.  A relationship between the detection of size, rate, orientation and direction in the human visual system. , 1973, Vision research.

[9]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Simultaneous motion contrast: Velocity, sensitivity and depth response , 1975, Vision Research.

[10]  M. A. Bouman,et al.  Perimetry of contrast detection thresholds of moving spatial sine wave patterns. III. The target extent as a sensitivity controlling parameter. , 1978, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[11]  Russell L. DeValois,et al.  Early Visual Processing: Feature Detection or Spatial Filtering? , 1982 .

[12]  D. H. Kelly Motion and vision. II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[13]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Motion processing in peripheral vision: Reaction time and perceived velocity , 1982, Vision Research.

[14]  A Pantle Adaptation to pattern spatial frequency: effects on visual movement sensitivity in humans. , 1970, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[15]  Peter Thompson,et al.  Velocity after-effects: The effects of adaptation to moving stimuli on the perception of subsequently seen moving stimuli , 1981, Vision Research.

[16]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Line length detectors in the human visual system: evidence from selective adaptation. , 1972, Vision research.

[17]  C. Schor,et al.  Spatial tuning of static and dynamic local stereopsis , 1984, Vision Research.

[18]  S. Klein,et al.  Detection and discrimination of the direction of motion in central and peripheral vision of normal and amblyopic observers , 1984, Vision Research.

[19]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  The analysis of the drift rate of moving sinusoidal gratings. , 1973, Vision research.

[20]  Gerald Westheimer,et al.  Spatial phase sensitivity for sinusoidal grating targets , 1978, Vision Research.