Grist and mills: on the cultural origins of cultural learning

Cumulative cultural evolution is what ‘makes us odd’; our capacity to learn facts and techniques from others, and to refine them over generations, plays a major role in making human minds and lives radically different from those of other animals. In this article, I discuss cognitive processes that are known collectively as ‘cultural learning’ because they enable cumulative cultural evolution. These cognitive processes include reading, social learning, imitation, teaching, social motivation and theory of mind. Taking the first of these three types of cultural learning as examples, I ask whether and to what extent these cognitive processes have been adapted genetically or culturally to enable cumulative cultural evolution. I find that recent empirical work in comparative psychology, developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience provides surprisingly little evidence of genetic adaptation, and ample evidence of cultural adaptation. This raises the possibility that it is not only ‘grist’ but also ‘mills’ that are culturally inherited; through social interaction in the course of development, we not only acquire facts about the world and how to deal with it (grist), we also build the cognitive processes that make ‘fact inheritance’ possible (mills).

[1]  C. Heyes,et al.  Imitation in infancy: the wealth of the stimulus. , 2011, Developmental science.

[2]  C. Heyes,et al.  Sensorimotor experience enhances automatic imitation of robotic action , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[3]  C. Heyes Automatic imitation. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  K. Laland,et al.  Identification of the Social and Cognitive Processes Underlying Human Cumulative Culture , 2012, Science.

[5]  H. Cheung,et al.  Early orthographic experience modifies both phonological awareness and on-line speech processing , 2004 .

[6]  J. Templeton,et al.  Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: the pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) and the Clark's nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana). , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[7]  D. Buss,et al.  Evolutionary psychology. Controversies, questions, prospects, and limitations. , 2010, The American psychologist.

[8]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  M. Seligman Phobias and preparedness , 1971 .

[10]  Andrew G. Young,et al.  The hidden structure of overimitation , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Making Mirrors: Premotor Cortex Stimulation Enhances Mirror and Counter-mirror Motor Facilitation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Cecilia Heyes,et al.  Acquisition of automatic imitation is sensitive to sensorimotor contingency. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  C. Heyes WHAT CAN IMITATION DO FOR COOPERATION ? , 2010 .

[14]  Louis Lefebvre,et al.  Song complexity correlates with learning ability in zebra finch males , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  ‘Goals’ are not an integral component of imitation , 2010, Cognition.

[16]  Louis Lefebvre,et al.  CHAPTER 6 – Is Social Learning an Adaptive Specialization? , 1996 .

[17]  E. Phelps,et al.  Social learning of fear , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  C. H. WADDINGTON,et al.  Canalization of Development and Genetic Assimilation of Acquired Characters , 1959, Nature.

[19]  C. Heyes,et al.  Experience modulates automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[20]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  Michael Balter Animal cognition. 'Killjoys' challenge claims of clever animals. , 2012, Science.

[22]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions , 2005 .

[23]  C. Heyes What's social about social learning? , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[24]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  fMRI Evidence of ‘Mirror’ Responses to Geometric Shapes , 2012, PloS one.

[25]  S. Mineka,et al.  Observational conditioning of snake fear in unrelated rhesus monkeys. , 1985, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[26]  C. Heyes,et al.  SOCIAL LEARNING IN ANIMALS: CATEGORIES AND MECHANISMS , 1994, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[27]  P. Richerson,et al.  Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. , 2004 .

[28]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Rethinking ‘Rational Imitation’ in 14-Month-Old Infants: A Perceptual Distraction Approach , 2012, PloS one.

[29]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Social learning and innovation are positively correlated in pigeons (Columba livia) , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[30]  A. Glenberg Introduction to the Mirror Neuron Forum , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[31]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[32]  Mark W Woolrich,et al.  Associative learning of social value , 2008, Nature.

[33]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[34]  D. Premack Why Humans Are Unique: Three Theories , 2010, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[35]  Positions in the Mirror Are Closer Than They Appear , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[36]  B. Galef,et al.  Demonstrator influence on observer diet preference: Analyses of critical social interactions and olfactory signals , 1985 .

[37]  Johannes C. Ziegler,et al.  Orthographic effects in spoken language: On-line activation or phonological restructuring? , 2009, Brain Research.

[38]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[39]  Rachael E. Jack,et al.  Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal , 2009, Current Biology.

[40]  John C. Avise,et al.  The Cognitive Niche: Coevolution of Intelligence, Sociality, and Language , 2010 .

[41]  M. Brass,et al.  Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Michael L. Anderson Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[43]  E. Jablonka,et al.  The co-evolution of language and emotions , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[44]  G. Csibra,et al.  Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  C. Heyes Causes and consequences of imitation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  R. Dolan,et al.  How the Opinion of Others Affects Our Valuation of Objects , 2010, Current Biology.

[47]  N. Shea New thinking, innateness and inherited representation , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[48]  J. Lanzetta,et al.  Influence of facial expressions on the classical conditioning of fear. , 1980, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  S. Dehaene,et al.  How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language , 2010, Science.

[50]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.

[51]  Dearborn Animal Intelligence: An Experimental Study of the Associative Processes in Animals , 1900 .

[52]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  • THE HUMAN INFANT AS “HOMO IMITANS” , 2013 .

[53]  P. Richerson,et al.  Culture and the Evolutionary Process , 1988 .

[54]  C. Heyes Where do mirror neurons come from? , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[55]  Martin H. Levinson Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution , 2006 .

[56]  M. Anisfeld,et al.  Only Tongue Protrusion Modeling Is Matched by Neonates , 1996 .

[57]  J. Bruner,et al.  Cultural learning. Author's reply , 1993 .

[58]  C. Heyes Four routes of cognitive evolution. , 2003, Psychological review.

[59]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[60]  E. Wilson,et al.  Sociobiology: The New Synthesis , 1975 .

[61]  Susan S. Jones,et al.  Exploration or imitation? The effect of music on 4-week-old infants' tongue protrusions. , 2006, Infant behavior & development.

[62]  B. Kenward Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[63]  P. Klopfer Observational Learning in Birds: the Establishment of Behavioral Modes , 1961 .

[64]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Ape and Human Cognition , 2010 .

[65]  K. Sterelny Language, gesture, skill: the co-evolutionary foundations of language , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[66]  D. Chivers,et al.  Damselfly larvae learn to recognize predators from chemical cues in the predator's diet , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[67]  L. Huber,et al.  Social learning in a non-social reptile (Geochelone carbonaria) , 2010, Biology Letters.

[68]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System , 2007, Current Biology.