Age-related effects in interlimb practice on coding complex movement sequences.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Stefan Panzer | Udo Fries | Melanie Krueger | Charles H Shea | Stefan Panzer | C. Shea | Nicole Gruetzmacher | N. Gruetzmacher | U. Fries | Melanie Krueger
[1] W. Verwey. Effect of Sequence Length on the Execution of Familiar Keying Sequences: Lasting Segmentation and Preparation? , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.
[2] J. Summers,et al. Aging effects on event and emergent timing in bimanual coordination. , 2010, Human movement science.
[3] D. Howard,et al. Implicit sequence learning: effects of level of structure, adult age, and extended practice. , 2004, Psychology and aging.
[4] Willem B. Verwey,et al. Evidence for the development of concurrent processing in a sequential keypressing task , 1994 .
[5] Stefan Panzer,et al. The transfer of movement sequences: Effects of decreased and increased load , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[6] Charles H Shea,et al. Age-related effects in sequential motor learning. , 2006, Physical therapy.
[7] D. Willingham,et al. Deficit in learning of a motor skill requiring strategy, but not of perceptuomotor recalibration, with aging. , 1998, Learning & memory.
[8] Charles H Shea,et al. Effect of Practice on Effector Independence , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.
[9] George E Stelmach,et al. Age-related kinematic differences as influenced by task difficulty, target size, and movement amplitude. , 2002, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.
[10] Daniel B. Willingham,et al. A Neuropsychological Theory of Motor Skill Learning , 2004 .
[11] James H. Howard,et al. Implicit sequence learning without motor sequencing in young and old adults , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[12] J. H. Howard,et al. Adult age differences in the rate of learning serial patterns: evidence from direct and indirect tests. , 1992, Psychology and aging.
[13] Timothy A. Salthouse,et al. Aging and time-sharing aspects of executive control , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[14] D. Rosenbaum. On choosing between movement sequences: comments on Rose (1988). , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] Stefan Panzer,et al. The effects of sequence difficulty and practice on proportional and nonproportional transfer , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[16] J. Patton,et al. Single Limb Performance Following Contralateral Bimanual Limb Training , 2007, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.
[17] T. Salthouse. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.
[18] C. Shea,et al. Transfer of movement sequences: bigger is better. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[19] D. Howard,et al. Age differences in learning serial patterns: direct versus indirect measures. , 1989, Psychology and Aging.
[20] K. Doya,et al. Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.
[21] D J Povel,et al. Structural factors in patterned finger tapping. , 1982, Acta psychologica.
[22] Claudia Voelcker-Rehage,et al. Effect of motor practice on dual-task performance in older adults. , 2007, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.
[23] Stefan Panzer,et al. The learning of two similar complex movement sequences: does practice insulate a sequence from interference? , 2008, Human movement science.
[24] John W. Krakauer,et al. Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[25] Willem B. Verwey,et al. Representations underlying skill in the discrete sequence production task: effect of hand used and hand position , 2008, Psychological research.
[26] Charles H. Shea,et al. Proportional and nonproportional transfer of movement sequences , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[27] Otmar Bock,et al. Sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly humans , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[28] Michael I. Jordan,et al. The Organization of Action Sequences: Evidence From a Relearning Task. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.
[29] K Y Haaland,et al. Skill learning in the elderly: diminished implicit and explicit memory for a motor sequence. , 1992, Psychology and aging.
[30] Peter S. Lum,et al. Greater reliance on impedance control in the nondominant arm compared with the dominant arm when adapting to a novel dynamic environment , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] Joachim Hoffmann,et al. Effector-related sequence learning in a bimanual-bisequential serial reaction time task , 2008, Psychological research.
[32] Willem B. Verwey,et al. Evidence for a multistage model of practice in a sequential movement task. , 1999 .
[33] S. Keele,et al. The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. , 2003, Psychological review.
[34] David A. Caulton,et al. On the Modularity of Sequence Representation , 1995 .
[35] Á. Pascual-Leone,et al. The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during sequence learning is specific for spatial information. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[36] Stefan Panzer,et al. Inter-manual transfer and practice: coding of simple motor sequences. , 2009, Acta psychologica.
[37] Claudia Voelcker-Rehage,et al. Motor-skill learning in older adults—a review of studies on age-related differences , 2008, European Review of Aging and Physical Activity.
[38] H. Kay. Learning of a Serial Task by Different Age Groups , 1951 .
[39] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[40] Benjamin A Clegg,et al. Effector dependent sequence learning in the serial RT task , 2005, Psychological research.
[41] Alexander M. Harner,et al. Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[42] R. Seidler,et al. Age-related declines in visuospatial working memory correlate with deficits in explicit motor sequence learning. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[43] A. Welford. Between bodily changes and performance: some possible reasons for slowing with age. , 1984, Experimental aging research.
[44] Digby Elliott,et al. Are there age-related differences in learning to optimize speed, accuracy, and energy expenditure? , 2007, Human movement science.
[45] C. Shea,et al. Schema Theory: A Critical Appraisal and Reevaluation , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.
[46] C. Winstein,et al. The locus of age-related movement slowing: sensory processing in continuous goal-directed aiming. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.
[47] Willem B. Verwey,et al. Diminished motor skill development in elderly: indications for limited motor chunk use. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[48] S. Glover,et al. Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[49] C H Shea,et al. Contextual dependencies: influence on response latency. , 1995, Memory.
[50] Charles H Shea,et al. Sequence Learning: Response Structure and Effector Transfer , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[51] Charles H Shea,et al. The coding and effector transfer of movement sequences. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[52] L Proteau,et al. Aging and motor control. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.
[53] W B Verwey,et al. Concatenating familiar movement sequences: the versatile cognitive processor. , 2001, Acta psychologica.
[54] Sunbin Song,et al. Evidence for parallel explicit and implicit sequence learning systems in older adults , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.
[55] Stefan Panzer,et al. Short article: Effects of interlimb practice on coding and learning of movement sequences , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[56] Kenji Doya,et al. fMRI investigation of cortical and subcortical networks in the learning of abstract and effector-specific representations of motor sequences , 2006, NeuroImage.
[57] Charles H. Shea,et al. Representation of movement sequences is related to task characteristics. , 2009, Acta psychologica.
[58] L. Boyd,et al. Multidimensional Motor Sequence Learning Is Impaired in Older But Not Younger or Middle-Aged Adults , 2008, Physical Therapy.
[59] A. Wingfield,et al. Hearing Loss in Older Adulthood , 2005 .
[60] Stefan Panzer,et al. Asymmetric effector transfer of complex movement sequences. , 2010, Human movement science.
[61] Andrew J. Kelly,et al. Implicit learning of predictive relationships in three-element visual sequences by young and old adults. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.