Parallel development of ERP and behavioural measurements of visual segmentation.

Visual segmentation, a process in which elements are integrated into a form and segregated from the background, is known to differ from adults at infancy. The further developmental trajectory of this process, and of the underlying brain mechanisms, during childhood and adolescence is unknown. The aim of the study was to investigate the developmental trajectory of ERP reflections of visual segmentation, and to relate this to behavioural performance. One hundred and eleven typically developing children from 7 to 18 years of age were divided into six age groups. Each child performed two visual tasks. In a texture segmentation task, the difference in event-related potential (ERP) response to homogeneous (no visual segmentation) and checkered stimuli (visual segmentation) was investigated. In addition, behavioural performance on integration of elements into contours was measured. Both behavioural and ERP measurements of visual segmentation differed from adults in 7-12 year-old children. Behaviourally, young children were less able to integrate elements into a contour than older children. In addition, a developmental change was present in the ERP pattern evoked by homogeneous versus checkered stimuli. The largest differences in behaviour and ERPs were found between 7-8- and 9-10-, and between 11-12- and 13-14-year-old children, indicating the strongest development between those age groups. Behavioural as well as ERP measurements at 13-14 years of age showed similar results to those of adults. These results reveal that visual segmentation continues to develop until early puberty. Only by 13-14 years of age, children do integrate and segregate visual information as adults do. These results can be interpreted in terms of functional connectivity within the visual cortex.

[1]  Marina Schmid,et al.  An Introduction To The Event Related Potential Technique , 2016 .

[2]  Clara Casco,et al.  A visual evoked potential correlate of global figure-ground segmentation , 1999, Vision Research.

[3]  D. Field,et al.  Integration of contours: new insights , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies , 2001, Brain Research Bulletin.

[5]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Texture segregation is processed by primary visual cortex in man and monkey. Evidence from VEP experiments , 1992, Vision Research.

[6]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  M. J. Van der Smagt,et al.  Keep Your Eyes on Development: The Behavioral and Neurophysiological Development of Visual Mechanisms Underlying Form Processing , 2012, Front. Psychiatry.

[8]  H. Spekreijse,et al.  Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1) , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Vasily A. Vakorin,et al.  Variability of Brain Signals Processed Locally Transforms into Higher Connectivity with Brain Development , 2011, Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  M. Roy,et al.  Development of visual texture segregation during the first year of life: a high-density electrophysiological study , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  R. F Hess,et al.  Contour integration and cortical processing , 2003, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[12]  R. von der Heydt,et al.  Analysis of the Context Integration Mechanisms Underlying Figure–Ground Organization in the Visual Cortex , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Ragland,et al.  Optimization and validation of a visual integration test for schizophrenia research. , 2012, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[14]  H. Engeland,et al.  A new approach to the study of detail perception in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): Investigating visual feedforward, horizontal and feedback processing , 2009, Vision Research.

[15]  Jean Bennett,et al.  Lateral Connectivity and Contextual Interactions in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[16]  I. Kovács Human development of perceptual organization , 2000, Vision Research.

[17]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Intrinsic connectivity and receptive field properties in visual cortex , 1985, Vision Research.

[18]  J. Lund,et al.  Intrinsic laminar lattice connections in primate visual cortex , 1983, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[19]  I. Kovács,et al.  Late maturation of visual spatial integration in humans. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  V. Lamme,et al.  The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Circuits for Local and Global Signal Integration in Primary Visual Cortex , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  I. Kovács,et al.  Perceptual Learning in Williams Syndrome: Looking Beyond Averages , 2012, PloS one.

[23]  Michael Bach,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of human texture segregation, an overview , 1998, Documenta Ophthalmologica.

[24]  Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al.  Feedforward and Recurrent Processing in Scene Segmentation: Electroencephalography and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Daphne Maurer,et al.  The effects of spatial proximity and collinearity on contour integration in adults and children , 2010, Vision Research.

[26]  S. Dain,et al.  Transient VEP and psychophysical chromatic contrast thresholds in children and adults , 2007, Vision Research.

[27]  Ruxandra Sireteanu,et al.  Texture segregation in infants and children , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[28]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Feedforward, horizontal, and feedback processing in the visual cortex , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[29]  H. Spekreijse,et al.  FigureGround Segregation in a Recurrent Network Architecture , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  Victor A. F. Lamme The neurophysiology of figure-ground segregation in primary visual cortex , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  Michael Bach,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of texture segregation in the human visual evoked potential , 1992, Vision Research.

[32]  C. Gilbert Adult cortical dynamics. , 1998, Physiological reviews.

[33]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[34]  E. Callaway,et al.  Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[35]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Separate processing dynamics for texture elements, boundaries and surfaces in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  P. Sajda,et al.  Extraclassical receptive field phenomena and short-range connectivity in V1. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  Chantal Kemner,et al.  A neural substrate for atypical low-level visual processing in autism spectrum disorder. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Contextual Modulation in Primary Visual Cortex , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  I Kovács,et al.  A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one: effect of closure in figure-ground segmentation. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Integrity of lateral and feedbackward connections in visual processing in children with pervasive developmental disorder , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  Kaustubh Supekar,et al.  Development of Large-Scale Functional Brain Networks in Children , 2009, NeuroImage.