Antisaccade error rates and gap effects in psychosis syndromes from bipolar-schizophrenia network for intermediate phenotypes 2 (B-SNIP2)

Abstract Background Antisaccade tasks can be used to index cognitive control processes, e.g. attention, behavioral inhibition, working memory, and goal maintenance in people with brain disorders. Though diagnoses of schizophrenia (SZ), schizoaffective (SAD), and bipolar I with psychosis (BDP) are typically considered to be distinct entities, previous work shows patterns of cognitive deficits differing in degree, rather than in kind, across these syndromes. Methods Large samples of individuals with psychotic disorders were recruited through the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes 2 (B-SNIP2) study. Anti- and pro-saccade task performances were evaluated in 189 people with SZ, 185 people with SAD, 96 people with BDP, and 279 healthy comparison participants. Logistic functions were fitted to each group's antisaccade speed-performance tradeoff patterns. Results Psychosis groups had higher antisaccade error rates than the healthy group, with SZ and SAD participants committing 2 times as many errors, and BDP participants committing 1.5 times as many errors. Latencies on correctly performed antisaccade trials in SZ and SAD were longer than in healthy participants, although error trial latencies were preserved. Parameters of speed-performance tradeoff functions indicated that compared to the healthy group, SZ and SAD groups had optimal performance characterized by more errors, as well as less benefit from prolonged response latencies. Prosaccade metrics did not differ between groups. Conclusions With basic prosaccade mechanisms intact, the higher speed-performance tradeoff cost for antisaccade performance in psychosis cases indicates a deficit that is specific to the higher-order cognitive aspects of saccade generation.

[1]  G. Pearlson,et al.  Distinguishing patterns of impairment on inhibitory control and general cognitive ability among bipolar with and without psychosis, schizophrenia, and schizoaffective disorder , 2020, Schizophrenia Research.

[2]  P. Knox,et al.  Age-related alterations in inhibitory control investigated using the minimally delayed oculomotor response task , 2020, PeerJ.

[3]  J. Whitwell,et al.  Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging , 2018, Current opinion in neurology.

[4]  Lauren E. Ethridge,et al.  Identification of Distinct Psychosis Biotypes Using Brain-Based Biomarkers. , 2018, Focus.

[5]  J. Egeland,et al.  Category fluency in schizophrenia: opposing effects of negative and positive symptoms? , 2018, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[6]  Brian C Coe,et al.  Mechanisms of saccade suppression revealed in the anti-saccade task , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  Jonathon Love,et al.  A Bayesian approach for estimating the probability of trigger failures in the stop-signal paradigm , 2016, Behavior research methods.

[8]  Justin B. Miller,et al.  Estimates of premorbid ability in a neurodegenerative disease clinic population: comparing the Test of Premorbid Functioning and the Wide Range Achievement Test, 4th Edition , 2016, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[9]  Jordan E. Pierce,et al.  Modulation of cognitive control levels via manipulation of saccade trial-type probability assessed with event-related BOLD fMRI. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Michael F. Green,et al.  Robust differences in antisaccade performance exist between COGS schizophrenia cases and controls regardless of recruitment strategies , 2015, Schizophrenia Research.

[11]  Lauren E. Ethridge,et al.  Behavioral response inhibition in psychotic disorders: Diagnostic specificity, familiality and relation to generalized cognitive deficit , 2014, Schizophrenia Research.

[12]  Godfrey D Pearlson,et al.  Elevated antisaccade error rate as an intermediate phenotype for psychosis across diagnostic categories. , 2014, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[13]  G. D. Pearlson,et al.  Resting-state brain function in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar probands and their first-degree relatives , 2014, Psychological Medicine.

[14]  Godfrey D Pearlson,et al.  Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder: findings from the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) study. , 2013, The American journal of psychiatry.

[15]  G. Pearlson,et al.  Clinical phenotypes of psychosis in the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP). , 2013, The American journal of psychiatry.

[16]  Melinda H. McCann,et al.  Using the Tukey-Kramer omnibus test in the Hayter-Fisher procedure. , 2012, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[17]  J. Dukart,et al.  Age Correction in Dementia – Matching to a Healthy Brain , 2011, PloS one.

[18]  Michael F. Green,et al.  Antisaccade performance in schizophrenia patients, their first-degree biological relatives, and community comparison subjects: data from the COGS study. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[19]  M. Thase,et al.  Response suppression deficits in treatment-naïve first-episode patients with schizophrenia, psychotic bipolar disorder and psychotic major depression , 2009, Psychiatry Research.

[20]  Paul M. Grant,et al.  Defeatist beliefs as a mediator of cognitive impairment, negative symptoms, and functioning in schizophrenia. , 2009, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[21]  K. Velanova,et al.  Development of eye-movement control , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[22]  B. Clementz,et al.  Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades: Evidence from studies of humans , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[23]  D. Ffytche,et al.  Decomposing the neural correlates of antisaccade eye movements using event-related FMRI. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  John A. Sweeney,et al.  Reduced Attentional Engagement Contributes to Deficits in Prefrontal Inhibitory Control in Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[25]  J. Barton,et al.  Schizophrenia patients show intact immediate error-related performance adjustments on an antisaccade task , 2006, Schizophrenia Research.

[26]  Matcheri S Keshavan,et al.  Longitudinal studies of antisaccades in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia , 2006, Psychological Medicine.

[27]  A. Sereno,et al.  Disruption of Reflexive Attention and Eye Movements in an Individual with a Collicular Lesion , 2006, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[28]  A. Beck,et al.  The Negative Symptoms of Schizophrenia: A Cognitive Perspective , 2005, Canadian journal of psychiatry. Revue canadienne de psychiatrie.

[29]  Josef C. Schrock,et al.  Working memory capacity and the antisaccade task: individual differences in voluntary saccade control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  D. Gooding,et al.  Temporal stability of saccadic task performance in schizophrenia and bipolar patients , 2004, Psychological Medicine.

[31]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Deficits in saccade target selection after inactivation of superior colliculus , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Philip D. Harvey,et al.  The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery , 2004, Schizophrenia Research.

[33]  R Bell,et al.  Effects of Amisulpride, Risperidone and Chlorpromazine on Auditory and Visual Latent Inhibition, Prepulse Inhibition, Executive Function and Eye Movements in Healthy Volunteers , 2004, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[34]  M. Segraves,et al.  Macaque frontal eye field input to saccade-related neurons in the superior colliculus. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  T. Mergner,et al.  Relationship between saccadic eye movements and cortical activity as measured by fMRI: quantitative and qualitative aspects , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  C. Bell Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision: DSM-IV-TR Quick Reference to the Diagnostic Criteria from DSM-IV-TR , 2001 .

[37]  K. Tallent,et al.  The association between antisaccade task and working memory task performance in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. , 2001, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[38]  R. Shafiq-Antonacci,et al.  The effects of age and mood on saccadic function in older individuals. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[39]  B. J. McCurtain,et al.  Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided saccades in humans: an fMRI study. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  G. Pearlson,et al.  Eye Movements and Psychopathology in Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder , 1996, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[41]  Michael F. Green,et al.  What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? , 1996, The American journal of psychiatry.

[42]  M. Birchwood,et al.  programmes with schizophrenic patients. new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention The Social Functioning Scale. The development and validation of a , 2022 .

[43]  C. K. Peck,et al.  Neuronal activity related to head and eye movements in cat superior colliculus. , 1990, The Journal of physiology.

[44]  R. M. Durand,et al.  Redundancy analysis: An alternative to canonical correlation and multivariate multiple regression in exploring interset associations. , 1988 .

[45]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  S. Kay,et al.  The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. , 1987, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[47]  M. Åsberg,et al.  A New Depression Scale Designed to be Sensitive to Change , 1979, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[48]  R. C. Young,et al.  A Rating Scale for Mania: Reliability, Validity and Sensitivity , 1978, British Journal of Psychiatry.