Landmarks or panoramas: what do navigating ants attend to for guidance?

BackgroundInsects are known to rely on terrestrial landmarks for navigation. Landmarks are used to chart a route or pinpoint a goal. The distant panorama, however, is often thought not to guide navigation directly during a familiar journey, but to act as a contextual cue that primes the correct memory of the landmarks.ResultsWe provided Melophorus bagoti ants with a huge artificial landmark located right near the nest entrance to find out whether navigating ants focus on such a prominent visual landmark for homing guidance. When the landmark was displaced by small or large distances, ant routes were affected differently. Certain behaviours appeared inconsistent with the hypothesis that guidance was based on the landmark only. Instead, comparisons of panoramic images recorded on the field, encompassing both landmark and distal panorama, could explain most aspects of the ant behaviours.ConclusionAnts navigating along a familiar route do not focus on obvious landmarks or filter out distal panoramic cues, but appear to be guided by cues covering a large area of their panoramic visual field, including both landmarks and distal panorama. Using panoramic views seems an appropriate strategy to cope with the complexity of natural scenes and the poor resolution of insects' eyes. The ability to isolate landmarks from the rest of a scene may be beyond the capacity of animals that do not possess a dedicated object-perception visual stream like primates.

[1]  Paul Graham,et al.  Visual Cues for the Retrieval of Landmark Memories by Navigating Wood Ants , 2007, Current Biology.

[2]  Paul Graham,et al.  The influence of beacon-aiming on the routes of wood ants , 2003, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[3]  T. S. Collett,et al.  The retrieval of visuo-spatial memories by honeybees , 2004, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[4]  Rüdiger Wehner,et al.  Piloting in desert ants: pinpointing the goal by discrete landmarks , 2010, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[5]  Rüdiger Wehner,et al.  Idiosyncratic route-based memories in desert ants, Melophorus bagoti: How do they interact with path-integration vectors? , 2005, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[6]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[7]  N. Tinbergen,et al.  Über die Orientierung des Bienenwolfes (Philanthus triangulum Fabr.) , 1938, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[8]  J. Zeil,et al.  The properties of the visual system in the Australian desert ant Melophorus bagoti. , 2011, Arthropod structure & development.

[9]  Allen Cheung,et al.  The information content of panoramic images I: The rotational errors and the similarity of views in rectangular experimental arenas. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[10]  J. Zeil Orientation flights of solitary wasps (Cerceris; Sphecidae; Hymenoptera) , 1993, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[11]  Ajay Narendra,et al.  Homing strategies of the Australian desert ant Melophorus bagoti II. Interaction of the path integrator with visual cue information , 2007, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[12]  Paul Graham,et al.  Image-matching during ant navigation occurs through saccade-like body turns controlled by learned visual features , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  G. Beugnon,et al.  Ant navigation en route to the goal: signature routes facilitate way-finding of Gigantiops destructor , 2006, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[14]  Matthew Collett,et al.  How desert ants use a visual landmark for guidance along a habitual route , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  R. Wehner,et al.  Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis: how to make a homing ant run away from home , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[16]  Antoine Wystrach,et al.  Geometry, features, and panoramic views: ants in rectangular arenas. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[17]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Motion cues provide the bee's visual world with a third dimension , 1988, Nature.

[18]  Antoine Wystrach,et al.  Views, landmarks, and routes: how do desert ants negotiate an obstacle course? , 2011, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[19]  T. Collett,et al.  Snapshot Memories and Landmark Guidance in Wood Ants , 2003, Current Biology.

[20]  Dimitrios Lambrinos,et al.  A neural model of landmark navigation in insects , 1999, Neurocomputing.

[21]  T. S. Collett,et al.  Landmark learning in bees , 1983, Journal of comparative physiology.

[22]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  Laura Dittmar Static and dynamic snapshots for goal localization in insects? , 2011, Communicative & integrative biology.

[24]  Ken Cheng,et al.  Learning, retention and coding of nest-associated visual cues by the Australian desert ant, Melophorus bagoti , 2007, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[25]  Isabelle Pastergue-Ruiz,et al.  Can the antCataglyphis cursor (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) encode global landmark-landmark relationships in addition to isolated landmark-goal relationships? , 2005, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[26]  N. Tinbergen Über die Orientierung des Bienenwolfes (Philanthus triangulum Fabr.) , 1932, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[27]  Paul Graham,et al.  Navigational Memories in Ants and Bees: Memory Retrieval When Selecting and Following Routes , 2006 .

[28]  R. Wehner,et al.  Visual spatial memory in desert ants,Cataglyphis bicolor (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) , 1979, Experientia.

[29]  J. Zeil,et al.  Polarised skylight and the landmark panorama provide night-active bull ants with compass information during route following , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[30]  T. Collett,et al.  Multiple stored views and landmark guidance in ants , 1998, Nature.

[31]  Antoine Wystrach,et al.  Ants in rectangular arenas , 2009, Communicative & integrative biology.

[32]  Andrew Vardy,et al.  Local visual homing by matched-filter descent in image distances , 2006, Biological Cybernetics.

[33]  G. A. Horridge,et al.  Recognition of a familiar place by the honeybee (Apis mellifera) , 2005, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[34]  Jochen Zeil,et al.  Catchment areas of panoramic snapshots in outdoor scenes. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[35]  T. Collett,et al.  Insect navigation en route to the goal: multiple strategies for the use of landmarks , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[36]  Sebastian Schwarz,et al.  Visual associative learning in two desert ant species , 2010, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[37]  Antoine Wystrach,et al.  Ants Learn Geometry and Features , 2009, Current Biology.

[38]  Ken Cheng Arthropod NavigationAnts, Bees, Crabs, Spiders Finding Their Way , 2012 .

[39]  Martin Egelhaaf,et al.  Goal seeking in honeybees: matching of optic flow snapshots? , 2010, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[40]  J. Zeil Orientation flights of solitary wasps (Cerceris; Sphecidae; Hymenoptera) , 1993, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[41]  Rüdiger Wehner,et al.  Visual navigation in desert ants Cataglyphis fortis: are snapshots coupled to a celestial system of reference? , 2002, The Journal of experimental biology.

[42]  T. Collett,et al.  The use of visual landmarks by honeybees: Bees weight landmarks according to their distance from the goal , 1987, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[43]  Thomas S. Collett,et al.  Memory use in insect visual navigation , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  Patrick Schultheiss,et al.  Finding the nest: inbound searching behaviour in the Australian desert ant, Melophorus bagoti , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[45]  P. Graham,et al.  Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation , 2009, Current Biology.

[46]  Thomas S. Collett,et al.  Landmark learning and guidance in insects , 1992 .

[47]  S. Al-Moghrabi,et al.  Inorganic carbon uptake for photosynthesis by the symbiotic coral-dinoflagellate association II. Mechanisms for bicarbonate uptake , 1996 .

[48]  E. C. Sobel The locust's use of motion parallax to measure distance , 1990, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[49]  T. Collett,et al.  Approaching and departing bees learn different cues to the distance of a landmark , 1994, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[50]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  The memory template in Drosophila pattern vision at the flight simulator , 1999, Vision Research.

[51]  T. S. Collett,et al.  Landmark maps for honeybees , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[52]  E. Wasserman,et al.  Comparative cognition : experimental explorations of animal intelligence , 2009 .

[53]  Paul Graham,et al.  The binding and recall of snapshot memories in wood ants (Formica rufa L.) , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.