Dishabituation of the BOLD response to speech sounds

BackgroundNeural systems show habituation responses at multiple levels, including relatively abstract language categories. Dishabituation – responses to non-habituated stimuli – can provide a window into the structure of these categories, without requiring an overt task.MethodsWe used an event-related fMRI design with short interval habituation trials, in which trains of stimuli were presented passively during 1.5 second intervals of relative silence between clustered scans. Trains of four identical stimuli (standard trials) and trains of three identical stimuli followed by a stimulus from a different phonetic category (deviant trials) were presented. This paradigm allowed us to measure and compare the time course of overall responses to speech, and responses to phonetic change.ResultsComparisons between responses to speech and silence revealed strong responses throughout the extent of superior temporal gyrus (STG) bilaterally. Comparisons between deviant and standard trials revealed dishabituation responses in a restricted region of left posterior STG, near the border with supramarginal gyrus (SMG). Novelty responses to deviant trials were also observed in right frontal regions and hippocampus.ConclusionA passive, dishabituation paradigm provides results similar to studies requiring overt responses. This paradigm can readily be extended for the study of pre-attentive processing of speech in populations such as children and second-language learners whose overt behavior is often difficult to interpret because of ancillary task demands.

[1]  I. Winkler,et al.  ‘Primitive intelligence’ in the auditory cortex , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[2]  K Mathiak,et al.  Rate-dependent activation of a prefrontal-insular-cerebellar network during passive listening to trains of click stimuli: an fMRI study , 2001, Neuroreport.

[3]  P. Matthews,et al.  Effective Paradigm Design , 2001 .

[4]  J. Desmond,et al.  Brain activation and sexual arousal in healthy, heterosexual males. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  Alison R Preston,et al.  Comparison of spiral-in/out and spiral-out BOLD fMRI at 1.5 and 3 T , 2004, NeuroImage.

[6]  G. Dehaene-Lambertz Electrophysiological correlates of categorical phoneme perception in adults , 1997, Neuroreport.

[7]  P. Deyn,et al.  The Lateralized Linguistic Cerebellum: A Review and a New Hypothesis , 2001, Brain and Language.

[8]  Jeffery A. Jones,et al.  Phonetic perceptual identification by native- and second-language speakers differentially activates brain regions involved with acoustic phonetic processing and those involved with articulatory–auditory/orosensory internal models , 2004, NeuroImage.

[9]  R Näätänen,et al.  Common neural mechanism for processing onset-to-onset intervals and silent gaps in sound sequences , 2001, Neuroreport.

[10]  P. Leppänen,et al.  Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants and children , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[11]  R. Goebel,et al.  Integration of Letters and Speech Sounds in the Human Brain , 2004, Neuron.

[12]  E. T. Possing,et al.  Human temporal lobe activation by speech and nonspeech sounds. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The Variability of Human, BOLD Hemodynamic Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[14]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  FMRI and PET of Self-Paced Finger Movement: Comparison of Intersubject Stereotaxic Averaged Data , 1999, NeuroImage.

[15]  H. Forssberg,et al.  Simultaneous movements of upper and lower limbs are coordinated by motor representations that are shared by both limbs: a PET study , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[16]  C. Best,et al.  Examination of perceptual reorganization for nonnative speech contrasts: Zulu click discrimination by English-speaking adults and infants. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  G Dehaene-Lambertz,et al.  A phonological representation in the infant brain , 1998, Neuroreport.

[18]  S. Scott,et al.  The neuroanatomical and functional organization of speech perception , 2003, Trends in Neurosciences.

[19]  Dominique Cardebat,et al.  Demand on verbal working memory delays haemodynamic response in the inferior prefrontal cortex , 2003, Human brain mapping.

[20]  K. Stephan,et al.  Control of action as mediated by the human frontal lobe , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Rapid Prefrontal-Hippocampal Habituation to Novel Events , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  B. Milner,et al.  A Cross-Linguistic PET Study of Tone Perception in Mandarin Chinese and English Speakers , 2001, NeuroImage.

[23]  S. Petersen,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of word reading. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  Irving Kupfermann,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of Habituation and Dishabituation of the Gill-Withdrawal Reflex in Aplysia , 1970, Science.

[25]  F. Chollet,et al.  Differential fMRI Responses in the Left Posterior Superior Temporal Gyrus and Left Supramarginal Gyrus to Habituation and Change Detection in Syllables and Tones , 1999, NeuroImage.

[26]  Alan R Palmer,et al.  Different areas of human non‐primary auditory cortex are activated by sounds with spatial and nonspatial properties , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[27]  Joseph S Gati,et al.  Overlapping neural regions for processing rapid temporal cues in speech and nonspeech signals☆ , 2003, NeuroImage.

[28]  E R Kandel,et al.  Neuronal Mechanisms of Habituation and Dishabituation of the Gill-Withdrawal Reflex in Aplysia , 1970, Science.

[29]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[30]  T. Hare,et al.  Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[31]  Guinevere F. Eden,et al.  Meta-Analysis of the Functional Neuroanatomy of Single-Word Reading: Method and Validation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[32]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Event-Related fMRI of the Auditory Cortex , 1998, NeuroImage.

[33]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses , 1997, Nature.

[34]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Phonological Grammar Shapes the Auditory Cortex: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  S. Bookheimer,et al.  Activation of language cortex with automatic speech tasks , 2000, Neurology.

[36]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Neural sources involved in auditory target detection and novelty processing: an event-related fMRI study. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[37]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex , 2000, Nature.

[38]  Michael W. L. Chee,et al.  Common and Segregated Neuronal Networks for Different Languages Revealed Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Adaptation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: IV. Some effects of perceptual learning on speech production. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[40]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Dishabituation of bold responses to phonetic oddballs an event-related fMRI study of magnitude of acoustic change and native language history , 2000 .

[41]  H. Scheich,et al.  Phonetic Perception and the Temporal Cortex , 2002, NeuroImage.

[42]  F. Craik,et al.  Novelty and familiarity activations in PET studies of memory encoding and retrieval. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  E. Schröger,et al.  Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fMRI and ERP Results , 2002, NeuroImage.

[44]  Willy Serniclaes,et al.  Neural correlates of switching from auditory to speech perception , 2005, NeuroImage.

[45]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  Jeffrey R. Binder,et al.  Simultaneous ERP and fMRI of the auditory cortex in a passive oddball paradigm , 2003, NeuroImage.

[47]  J. Movshon,et al.  Adaptation changes the direction tuning of macaque MT neurons , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Susceptibility-Induced Loss of Signal: Comparing PET and fMRI on a Semantic Task , 2000, NeuroImage.