Television and Very Young Children

The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) has recommended that children younger than 24 months of age not be exposed to television. Nevertheless, television programs and home videos are increasingly produced for very young children. This article reviews the extant research concerning television and very young children with respect to the AAP recommendation. More very young children are currently watching television than in the recent past; they pay substantial attention to TV programs and videos made for them. When learning from videos is assessed in comparison to equivalent live presentations, there is usually substantially less learning from videos. Although one study finds positive associations of language learning with exposure to some children’s TV programs, other studies find negative associations of viewing with language, cognitive, and attentional development. Background TV is also a disruptive influence. Evidence thus far indicates that the AAP recommendation is well taken, although considerably more research is needed.

[1]  Shalom M. Fisch,et al.  "G" Is for Growing: Thirty Years of Research on Children and "Sesame Street.". , 2001 .

[2]  R. Kahn,et al.  Prevalence, correlates, and trajectory of television viewing among infants and toddlers. , 2002, Pediatrics.

[3]  H. Hayne,et al.  Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. , 1999, Child development.

[4]  T. Suddendorf Early representational insight: twenty-four-month-olds can use a photo to find an object in the world. , 2003, Child development.

[5]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Viewing the viewers: Viewing behaviors by children and adults during television programs and commercials , 2003 .

[6]  E. Lorch,et al.  Television viewing at home: age trends in visual attention and time with TV. , 1986, Child development.

[7]  L. Frank The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .

[8]  Dimitri A Christakis,et al.  Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children. , 2004, Pediatrics.

[9]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Form and content: looking at visual features of television. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[10]  J. Carew Experience and the development of intelligence in young children at home and in day care. , 1980, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[11]  H. Hayne,et al.  Imitation from television by 24- and 30-month-olds. , 2003 .

[12]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Attentional inertia in children's extended looking at television. , 2004, Advances in child development and behavior.

[13]  S. Alessandri Mother-child interactional correlates of maltreated and nonmaltreated children's play behavior , 1992, Development and Psychopathology.

[14]  J. Sachs,et al.  Language learning with restricted input: Case studies of two hearing children of deaf parents , 1981, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[15]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Reality: Toddlers' Use of Visual Information from Video to Guide Behavior , 2002 .

[16]  Jennings Bryant,et al.  Children's Understanding of Television: Research on Attention and Comprehension , 1983 .

[17]  P. L. Adams THE ORIGINS OF INTELLIGENCE IN CHILDREN , 1976 .

[18]  Elizabeth Pugzles Lorch,et al.  The Effects of TV Program Comprehensibility on Preschool Children's Visual Attention to Television. , 1981 .

[19]  V. D. Sanua,et al.  The Annual Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association , 1958 .

[20]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Early childhood television viewing and adolescent behavior: the recontact study. , 2001, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[21]  Mabel L. Rice,et al.  Words from Sesame Street : learning vocabulary while viewing , 1990 .

[22]  J. Deloache,et al.  The medium can obscure the message: young children's understanding of video. , 1998, Child development.

[23]  V. Rideout,et al.  Zero to Six: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants, Toddlers and Preschoolers , 2003 .

[24]  L. DiLalla,et al.  Differentiation of Fantasy and Reality: Preschoolers' Reactions to Interruptions in Their Play. , 1988 .

[25]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Young children's attention to "Sesame Street." , 1976 .

[26]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  TV guide: two-year-old children learn to use video as a source of information. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[27]  Aletha C. Huston,et al.  The Forms of Television and the Child Viewer , 1989 .

[28]  Deborah L. Linebarger,et al.  Infants’ and Toddlers’ Television Viewing and Language Outcomes , 2005 .

[29]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  Marie Evans Schmidt,et al.  Two-Year-Olds' Object Retrieval Based on Television: Testing a Perceptual Account , 2007 .

[31]  M L Rice,et al.  Lessons from television: children's word learning when viewing. , 1988, Child development.

[32]  George Comstock,et al.  Public communication and behavior , 1986 .

[33]  E. Kako,et al.  First contact in verb acquisition: defining a role for syntax. , 1993, Child development.

[34]  K. Nelson,et al.  Structure and strategy in learning to talk. , 1973 .

[35]  D. Singer,et al.  Handbook of Children and the Media , 2000 .

[36]  D. L. Mumme,et al.  The infant as onlooker: learning from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. , 2003, Child development.

[37]  Patti M. Valkenburg,et al.  Developmental Changes in Infants’ and Toddlers’ Attention to Television Entertainment , 2004, Commun. Res..