Delayed Response to Animate Implied Motion in Human Motion Processing Areas

Viewing static photographs of objects in motion evokes higher fMRI activation in the human medial temporal complex (MT) than looking at similar photographs without this implied motion. As MT is traditionally thought to be involved in motion perception (and not in form perception), this finding suggests feedback from object-recognition areas onto MT. To investigate this hypothesis, we recorded extracranial potentials evoked by the sight of photographs of biological agents with and without implied motion. The difference in potential between responses to pictures with and without implied motion was maximal between 260 and 400 msec after stimulus onset. Source analysis of this difference revealed one bilateral, symmetrical dipole pair in the occipital lobe. This area also showed a response to real motion, but approximately 100 msec earlier than the implied motion response. The longer latency of the implied motion response in comparison to the real motion response is consistent with a feedback projection onto MT following object recognition in higher-level temporal areas.

[1]  E. Bullmore,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of implicit-motion perception or ‘representational momentum’ , 2000, Current Biology.

[2]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[3]  G. Mangun,et al.  Luminance and spatial attention effects on early visual processing. , 1995, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[4]  J. Freyd,et al.  The mental representation of movement when static stimuli are viewed , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  W. Paulus,et al.  Identification of the visual motion area (area V5) in the human brain by dipole source analysis , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  D. Perrett,et al.  Perceptual History Influences Neural Responses to Face and Body Postures , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Michael Bach,et al.  Directional tuning of human motion adaptation as reflected by the motion VEP , 2001, Vision Research.

[8]  S. Heinrich A primer on motion visual evoked potentials , 2007, Documenta Ophthalmologica.

[9]  Jeannette A. M. Lorteije,et al.  Adaptation to Real Motion Reveals Direction-selective Interactions between Real and Implied Motion Processing , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Paul Dassonville,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation over MT/MST fails to impair judgments of implied motion , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance Volume Xix , 2001 .

[12]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Activation in Human MT/MST by Static Images with Implied Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Moake,et al.  This article has been cited by other articles , 2003 .

[15]  G. Orban,et al.  Motion-responsive regions of the human brain , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Stefan Debener,et al.  Size matters: effects of stimulus size, duration and eccentricity on the visual gamma-band response , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[17]  G. R Mangun,et al.  On the processing of spatial frequencies as revealed by evoked-potential source modeling , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[18]  Frank Bremmer,et al.  Neural correlates of implied motion , 2003, Nature.

[19]  Tjeerd Jellema,et al.  Cells in monkey STS responsive to articulated body motions and consequent static posture: a case of implied motion? , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Z. Kourtzi,et al.  Linking form and motion in the primate brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  R. Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  Marina Pavlova,et al.  Perception and Understanding of Others' Actions and Brain Connectivity , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Representational momentum and the brain: An investigation into the functional necessity of V5/MT , 2002 .

[25]  T W Picton,et al.  Separation and identification of event-related potential components by brain electric source analysis. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[26]  S. Tobimatsu,et al.  Effects of stimulus orientation on spatial frequency function of the visual evoked potential , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  J. Winawer,et al.  A Motion Aftereffect From Still Photographs Depicting Motion , 2008, Psychological science.

[28]  R A Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. III. Modeling , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  Jeannette A M Lorteije,et al.  Disentangling neural structures for processing of high‐ and low‐speed visual motion , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[30]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.