PERCEPTUAL-MOTOR INTERACTION: SOME IMPLICATIONS FOR HCI

[1]  R. Klein,et al.  Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  A. Cuschieri,et al.  Task performance in endoscopic surgery is influenced by location of the image display. , 1998, Annals of surgery.

[3]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Action-centred attention in virtual environments. , 1999 .

[4]  Douglas C. Engelbart,et al.  Display-Selection Techniques for Text Manipulation , 1967 .

[5]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Object-based processes in the planning of goal-directed hand movements , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[6]  D. Elliott,et al.  Movement Trajectories in the Presence of a Distracting Stimulus: Evidence for a Response Activation Model of Selective Reaching , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[7]  Luc Tremblay,et al.  Between-trial inhibition and facilitation in goal-directed aiming: manual and spatial asymmetries , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Alphonse Chapanis,et al.  A Reaction Time Study of Four Control-Display Linkages1 , 1959 .

[9]  B. Schneiderman,et al.  Designing the User Interface. Strategies for Effective Human-Computer Interaction , 1992 .

[10]  R W Remington,et al.  The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Timothy N Welsh,et al.  Effects of response priming and inhibition on movement planning and execution. , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[12]  C. W. Telford The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .

[13]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  S. Tipper,et al.  Inhibition of return to successively cued spatial locations: commentary on Pratt and Abrams (1995). , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[16]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Within- and between-nervous-system inhibition of return: Observation is as good as performance , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[18]  W. E. Hick Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.

[19]  C. Umilta,et al.  Splitting visual space with attention. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Regan L. Mandryk,et al.  Superimposing display space on workspace in the context of endoscopic surgery , 1999, CHI Extended Abstracts.

[21]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .

[22]  Ben Shneiderman,et al.  Designing the User Interface: Strategies for Effective Human-Computer Interaction , 1998 .

[23]  W. Helsen,et al.  Temporal and spatial coupling of point of gaze and hand movements in aiming. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  Digby Elliott,et al.  The effects of response priming on the planning and execution of goal-directed movements in the presence of a distracting stimulus. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[25]  Vernon B. Brooks,et al.  Introductory lecture to session III some examples of programmed limb movements , 1974 .

[26]  Ben Shneiderman,et al.  Direct Manipulation: A Step Beyond Programming Languages , 1983, Computer.

[27]  J R Simon,et al.  Effect of ear stimulated on reaction time and movement time. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  Jae T Patterson,et al.  Learning a new human-computer alphabet: the role of similarity and practice. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[29]  David R. Millen,et al.  Consistent Layout of Function Keys and Screen Labels Speeds User Responses , 1988 .

[30]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  Jon Driver,et al.  Spreading of Visual Attention to Modally Versus Amodally Completed Regions , 1997 .

[32]  R. Marteniuk Information processing in motor skills , 1976 .

[33]  C Iani,et al.  The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Carlo Umiltà,et al.  Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect , 1997 .

[35]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming , 1991 .

[36]  R W Proctor,et al.  Referential coding and attention-shifting accounts of the Simon effect , 1994, Psychological research.

[37]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Human factors in simple and complex systems , 1993 .

[38]  J. Pratt,et al.  The effects of action video game experience on the time course of inhibition of return and the efficiency of visual search. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[39]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.

[40]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.

[41]  R. Hyman Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[42]  A. Allport Attention and control: have we been asking the wrong questions? A critical review of twenty-five years , 1993 .

[43]  R. Wallace,et al.  S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[44]  Stuart K. Card,et al.  Evaluation of mouse, rate-controlled isometric joystick, step keys, and text keys, for text selection on a CRT , 1987 .

[45]  Carlo Umiltà,et al.  An integrated model of the Simon effect. , 1992 .

[46]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[47]  P. Fitts,et al.  S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[48]  E. C. Cherry Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and with Two Ears , 1953 .

[49]  J. Duncan Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[50]  R. Rafal,et al.  Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[51]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[52]  E. Maylor,et al.  Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[53]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Saccades operate in violation of Hick’s law , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  A. Treisman VERBAL CUES, LANGUAGE, AND MEANING IN SELECTIVE ATTENTION. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[55]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Visual Search Is Modulated by Action Intentions , 2002, Psychological science.

[56]  H. J. Muller,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  Errol R. Hoffmann,et al.  Concurrent manual-decision tasks , 1997 .

[58]  Steven P. Tipper,et al.  Visual search and target-directed action , 1999 .

[59]  T. Stoffer Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility , 1991, Psychological research.

[60]  R. Chua,et al.  Influence of terminal action requirements on action-centered distractor effects , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[61]  M. Posner Cumulative Development of Attentional Theory. , 1982 .

[62]  R. Abrams,et al.  Motion Onset Captures Attention , 2003, Psychological science.

[63]  Carlo Umiltà,et al.  Attention shifts produce spatial stimulus codes , 1994, Psychological research.

[64]  A. T. Welford,et al.  The fundamentals of skill , 1968 .

[65]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[66]  J. Findlay,et al.  The Relationship between Eye Movements and Spatial Attention , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[67]  R. D. Wright,et al.  Inhibition-of-return at multiple locations in visual space. , 1996, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[68]  J. Pratt,et al.  Action-centered inhibition: Effects of distractors on movement planning and execution , 1994 .

[69]  D. Elliott,et al.  Hand deviations toward distractors Evidence for response competition , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[70]  D. Elliott,et al.  Visual regulation of manual aiming , 1993 .

[71]  D Goodman,et al.  On the coordination of two-handed movements. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[72]  J. Pratt,et al.  Inhibition of return to successively cued spatial locations. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[73]  A. Treisman THE EFFECT OF IRRELEVANT MATERIAL ON THE EFFICIENCY OF SELECTIVE LISTENING. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[74]  M. Cheal,et al.  Central and Peripheral Precuing of Forced-Choice Discrimination , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[75]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The relationship between inhibition of return and saccade trajectory deviations. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[76]  E. R. Crossman,et al.  Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[77]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[78]  W. Helsen,et al.  Coupling of Eye, Finger, Elbow, and Shoulder Movements During Manual Aiming , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[79]  A B Sekuler,et al.  The effects of occlusion and past experience on the allocation of object-based attention , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[80]  S. Tipper,et al.  Reaching into cluttered visual environments: spatial and temporal influences of distracting objects. , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[81]  J. Adams,et al.  A closed-loop theory of motor learning. , 1971, Journal of motor behavior.

[82]  D. Broadbent Perception and communication , 1958 .

[83]  Stephen J. Payne,et al.  Naive Judgments of Stimulus-Response Compatibility , 1995, Hum. Factors.

[84]  R A Abrams,et al.  Spatially diffuse inhibition affects multiple locations: a reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996). , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[85]  R. L. Deininger,et al.  S-R compatibility: correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[86]  Jay Pratt,et al.  The effects of onsets and offsets on visual attention , 2001, Psychological research.

[87]  Anne Treisman,et al.  Features and objects in visual processing , 1986 .

[88]  Robert J. K. Jacob,et al.  Evaluation of eye gaze interaction , 2000, CHI.

[89]  J. Deutsch,et al.  Attention: Some theoretical considerations. , 1963 .

[90]  Christine L. MacKenzie,et al.  Representational and Physical Accounts of Motor Control and Learning: Can they Account for the Data? , 1988 .

[91]  P. Calabresi,et al.  Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[92]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Does Joe influence Fred's action? Inhibition of return across different nervous systems , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[93]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[94]  Jon Driver,et al.  Inhibition of return is supramodal: a demonstration between all possible pairings of vision, touch, and audition , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[95]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .

[96]  U. Castiello Understanding other people's actions: intention and attention. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[97]  J. A. Adams,et al.  Short-term memory for motor responses. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[98]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[99]  E. Tenner Why things bite back : technology and the revenge of unintended consequences , 1996 .

[100]  R. Proctor,et al.  Naïve and experienced judgments of stimuluS—Response compatibility: implications for interface design , 2003, Ergonomics.

[101]  Steven P. Tipper,et al.  Action-centred negative priming: Evidence for reactive inhibition , 2002 .

[102]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[103]  S. Tipper,et al.  Hand deviations away from visual cues: Indirect evidence for inhibition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.