PERCEPTUAL-MOTOR INTERACTION: SOME IMPLICATIONS FOR HCI
暂无分享,去创建一个
Romeo Chua | Timothy N. Welsh | Romeo Chua | David Goodman | Daniel J. Weeks | D. Goodman | R. Chua | D. Weeks | T. Welsh | David Goodman
[1] R. Klein,et al. Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] A. Cuschieri,et al. Task performance in endoscopic surgery is influenced by location of the image display. , 1998, Annals of surgery.
[3] Romeo Chua,et al. Action-centred attention in virtual environments. , 1999 .
[4] Douglas C. Engelbart,et al. Display-Selection Techniques for Text Manipulation , 1967 .
[5] Harold Bekkering,et al. Object-based processes in the planning of goal-directed hand movements , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[6] D. Elliott,et al. Movement Trajectories in the Presence of a Distracting Stimulus: Evidence for a Response Activation Model of Selective Reaching , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[7] Luc Tremblay,et al. Between-trial inhibition and facilitation in goal-directed aiming: manual and spatial asymmetries , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[8] Alphonse Chapanis,et al. A Reaction Time Study of Four Control-Display Linkages1 , 1959 .
[9] B. Schneiderman,et al. Designing the User Interface. Strategies for Effective Human-Computer Interaction , 1992 .
[10] R W Remington,et al. The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] Timothy N Welsh,et al. Effects of response priming and inhibition on movement planning and execution. , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.
[12] C. W. Telford. The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .
[13] J. C. Johnston,et al. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] S. Tipper,et al. Inhibition of return to successively cued spatial locations: commentary on Pratt and Abrams (1995). , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[16] Romeo Chua,et al. Within- and between-nervous-system inhibition of return: Observation is as good as performance , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[17] A. Opstal. Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .
[18] W. E. Hick. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.
[19] C. Umilta,et al. Splitting visual space with attention. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] Regan L. Mandryk,et al. Superimposing display space on workspace in the context of endoscopic surgery , 1999, CHI Extended Abstracts.
[21] S. Tipper,et al. Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .
[22] Ben Shneiderman,et al. Designing the User Interface: Strategies for Effective Human-Computer Interaction , 1998 .
[23] W. Helsen,et al. Temporal and spatial coupling of point of gaze and hand movements in aiming. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.
[24] Digby Elliott,et al. The effects of response priming on the planning and execution of goal-directed movements in the presence of a distracting stimulus. , 2005, Acta psychologica.
[25] Vernon B. Brooks,et al. Introductory lecture to session III some examples of programmed limb movements , 1974 .
[26] Ben Shneiderman,et al. Direct Manipulation: A Step Beyond Programming Languages , 1983, Computer.
[27] J R Simon,et al. Effect of ear stimulated on reaction time and movement time. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[28] Jae T Patterson,et al. Learning a new human-computer alphabet: the role of similarity and practice. , 2005, Acta psychologica.
[29] David R. Millen,et al. Consistent Layout of Function Keys and Screen Labels Speeds User Responses , 1988 .
[30] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[31] Jon Driver,et al. Spreading of Visual Attention to Modally Versus Amodally Completed Regions , 1997 .
[32] R. Marteniuk. Information processing in motor skills , 1976 .
[33] C Iani,et al. The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] Carlo Umiltà,et al. Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect , 1997 .
[35] Romeo Chua,et al. Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming , 1991 .
[36] R W Proctor,et al. Referential coding and attention-shifting accounts of the Simon effect , 1994, Psychological research.
[37] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Human factors in simple and complex systems , 1993 .
[38] J. Pratt,et al. The effects of action video game experience on the time course of inhibition of return and the efficiency of visual search. , 2005, Acta psychologica.
[39] M. Jeannerod,et al. Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.
[40] J. R. Simon,et al. Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.
[41] R. Hyman. Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[42] A. Allport. Attention and control: have we been asking the wrong questions? A critical review of twenty-five years , 1993 .
[43] R. Wallace,et al. S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[44] Stuart K. Card,et al. Evaluation of mouse, rate-controlled isometric joystick, step keys, and text keys, for text selection on a CRT , 1987 .
[45] Carlo Umiltà,et al. An integrated model of the Simon effect. , 1992 .
[46] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[47] P. Fitts,et al. S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[48] E. C. Cherry. Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and with Two Ears , 1953 .
[49] J. Duncan. Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[50] R. Rafal,et al. Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[51] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[52] E. Maylor,et al. Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[53] Leanne Boucher,et al. Saccades operate in violation of Hick’s law , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[54] A. Treisman. VERBAL CUES, LANGUAGE, AND MEANING IN SELECTIVE ATTENTION. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.
[55] H. Bekkering,et al. Visual Search Is Modulated by Action Intentions , 2002, Psychological science.
[56] H. J. Muller,et al. Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] Errol R. Hoffmann,et al. Concurrent manual-decision tasks , 1997 .
[58] Steven P. Tipper,et al. Visual search and target-directed action , 1999 .
[59] T. Stoffer. Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility , 1991, Psychological research.
[60] R. Chua,et al. Influence of terminal action requirements on action-centered distractor effects , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[61] M. Posner. Cumulative Development of Attentional Theory. , 1982 .
[62] R. Abrams,et al. Motion Onset Captures Attention , 2003, Psychological science.
[63] Carlo Umiltà,et al. Attention shifts produce spatial stimulus codes , 1994, Psychological research.
[64] A. T. Welford,et al. The fundamentals of skill , 1968 .
[65] S. Tipper,et al. Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[66] J. Findlay,et al. The Relationship between Eye Movements and Spatial Attention , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[67] R. D. Wright,et al. Inhibition-of-return at multiple locations in visual space. , 1996, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[68] J. Pratt,et al. Action-centered inhibition: Effects of distractors on movement planning and execution , 1994 .
[69] D. Elliott,et al. Hand deviations toward distractors Evidence for response competition , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[70] D. Elliott,et al. Visual regulation of manual aiming , 1993 .
[71] D Goodman,et al. On the coordination of two-handed movements. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[72] J. Pratt,et al. Inhibition of return to successively cued spatial locations. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[73] A. Treisman. THE EFFECT OF IRRELEVANT MATERIAL ON THE EFFICIENCY OF SELECTIVE LISTENING. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.
[74] M. Cheal,et al. Central and Peripheral Precuing of Forced-Choice Discrimination , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[75] J. Theeuwes,et al. The relationship between inhibition of return and saccade trajectory deviations. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[76] E. R. Crossman,et al. Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[77] R. Ellis,et al. On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[78] W. Helsen,et al. Coupling of Eye, Finger, Elbow, and Shoulder Movements During Manual Aiming , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.
[79] A B Sekuler,et al. The effects of occlusion and past experience on the allocation of object-based attention , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[80] S. Tipper,et al. Reaching into cluttered visual environments: spatial and temporal influences of distracting objects. , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[81] J. Adams,et al. A closed-loop theory of motor learning. , 1971, Journal of motor behavior.
[82] D. Broadbent. Perception and communication , 1958 .
[83] Stephen J. Payne,et al. Naive Judgments of Stimulus-Response Compatibility , 1995, Hum. Factors.
[84] R A Abrams,et al. Spatially diffuse inhibition affects multiple locations: a reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996). , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[85] R. L. Deininger,et al. S-R compatibility: correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[86] Jay Pratt,et al. The effects of onsets and offsets on visual attention , 2001, Psychological research.
[87] Anne Treisman,et al. Features and objects in visual processing , 1986 .
[88] Robert J. K. Jacob,et al. Evaluation of eye gaze interaction , 2000, CHI.
[89] J. Deutsch,et al. Attention: Some theoretical considerations. , 1963 .
[90] Christine L. MacKenzie,et al. Representational and Physical Accounts of Motor Control and Learning: Can they Account for the Data? , 1988 .
[91] P. Calabresi,et al. Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[92] Romeo Chua,et al. Does Joe influence Fred's action? Inhibition of return across different nervous systems , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.
[93] R A Abrams,et al. Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.
[94] Jon Driver,et al. Inhibition of return is supramodal: a demonstration between all possible pairings of vision, touch, and audition , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[95] C. MacKenzie,et al. Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .
[96] U. Castiello. Understanding other people's actions: intention and attention. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[97] J. A. Adams,et al. Short-term memory for motor responses. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.
[98] H. Deubel,et al. Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.
[99] E. Tenner. Why things bite back : technology and the revenge of unintended consequences , 1996 .
[100] R. Proctor,et al. Naïve and experienced judgments of stimuluS—Response compatibility: implications for interface design , 2003, Ergonomics.
[101] Steven P. Tipper,et al. Action-centred negative priming: Evidence for reactive inhibition , 2002 .
[102] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[103] S. Tipper,et al. Hand deviations away from visual cues: Indirect evidence for inhibition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.