Visual information about moving objects.

A mathematical model of visual flow is presented, which could potentially account for an observer's ability to effectively interact with moving objects. The analysis demonstrates that there is visual information available to an observer about (a) a moving object's angle of approach, (b) changes in its velocity and acceleration, (c) whether its velocity and acceleration are positive or negative, (d) its time to collision with both the horizontal and vertical axes, and (e) whether it will cross the horizontal axis in front of or behind the point of observation. Several experiments are reported in which observer's sensitivity to these different aspects of visual information is examined using a variety of experimental tasks. The results suggest that human observers are highly sensitive to many abstract properties of visual stimulation, but that they are not sensitive to all of the information that is potentially available.

[1]  J S Lappin,et al.  Minimal conditions for the visual detection of structure and motion in three dimensions. , 1980, Science.

[2]  J. B. Pittenger,et al.  Perceptual information for the age level of faces as a higher order invariant of growth. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[4]  J. B. Pittenger,et al.  The perception of human growth. , 1980, Scientific American.

[5]  R. Warren The perception of egomotion. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Gottsdanker Rm,et al.  Prediction-motion with and without vision. , 1952 .

[7]  H. Flock SOME CONDITIONS SUFFICIENT FOR ACCURATE MONOCULAR PERCEPTIONS OF MOVING SURFACE SLANTS. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  W Schiff,et al.  Information Used in Judging Impending Collision , 1979, Perception.

[9]  J. B. Pittenger,et al.  Aging faces as viscal-elastic events: implications for a theory of nonrigid shape perception. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  James A. Caviness,et al.  Persistent Fear Responses in Rhesus Monkeys to the Optical Stimulus of "Looming" , 1962, Science.

[11]  David N. Lee,et al.  Visual control of locomotion. , 1977, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[12]  G. Johansson,et al.  Visual Perception of Bending Motion , 1973, Perception.

[13]  R. Gottsdanker,et al.  A further study of prediction-motion. , 1955, The American journal of psychology.

[14]  S. Ullman The Interpretation of Visual Motion , 1979 .

[15]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[16]  David N. Lee Visual proprioceptive control of stance , 1975 .

[17]  J. N. Bassili Facial motion in the perception of faces and of emotional expression. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Ebbe B. Ebbesen,et al.  Laboratory and field analyses of decisions involving risk. , 1977 .

[19]  John Martin Prager,et al.  Segmentation of static and dynamic scenes. , 1979 .

[20]  J. Gibson,et al.  Continuous perspective transformations and the perception of rigid motion. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  J. Gibson,et al.  Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. , 1955, The American journal of psychology.

[22]  R. Gottsdanker,et al.  Identifying the acceleration of visual targets. , 1961, British journal of psychology.

[23]  J. Gibson The perception of the visual world , 1951 .

[24]  R. Shaw,et al.  Simple kinetic information for transparent depth , 1974 .

[25]  G Jansson Perceived bending and stretching motions from a line of points. , 1977, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[26]  S. Runeson,et al.  Constant velocity — Not perceived as such , 1974, Psychological research.

[27]  J. Gibson,et al.  The sensitivity of the eye to two kinds of continuous transformation of a shadow-pattern. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Optical Velocity Patterns, Velocity-Sensitive Neurons, and Space Perception: A Hypothesis , 1974, Perception.

[29]  J. Gibson,et al.  Motion parallax as a determinant of perceived depth. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[30]  R. Gottsdanker The accuracy of prediction motion. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[31]  H. Wallach,et al.  The kinetic depth effect. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[33]  David N. Lee,et al.  A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.

[34]  G Jansson,et al.  Perceived Bending Motions from a Quadrangle Changing Form , 1977, Perception.

[35]  D A Gordon,et al.  Static and dynamic visul fields in human space perception. , 1965, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[36]  David N. Lee,et al.  Visual proprioceptive control of standing in human infants , 1974 .

[37]  G. Johansson Visual motion perception. , 1975, Scientific American.

[38]  J. Lishman,et al.  The Autonomy of Visual Kinaesthesis , 1973, Perception.

[39]  J E Cutting,et al.  A biomechanical invariant for gait perception. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  W. Schiff PERCEPTION OF IMPENDING COLLISION: A STUDY OF VISUALLY DIRECTED AVOIDANT BEHAVIOR. , 1965, Psychological monographs.

[41]  S. Runeson Visual prediction of collision with natural and nonnatural motion functions , 1975 .