Neural chronometry and coherency across speed–accuracy demands reveal lack of homomorphism between computational and neural mechanisms of evidence accumulation

The stochastic accumulation framework provides a mechanistic, quantitative account of perceptual decision-making and how task performance changes with experimental manipulations. Importantly, it provides an elegant account of the speed–accuracy trade-off (SAT), which has long been the litmus test for decision models, and also mimics the activity of single neurons in several key respects. Recently, we developed a paradigm whereby macaque monkeys trade speed for accuracy on cue during visual search task. Single-unit activity in frontal eye field (FEF) was not homomorphic with the architecture of models, demonstrating that stochastic accumulators are an incomplete description of neural activity under SAT. This paper summarizes and extends this work, further demonstrating that the SAT leads to extensive, widespread changes in brain activity never before predicted. We will begin by reviewing our recently published work that establishes how spiking activity in FEF accomplishes SAT. Next, we provide two important extensions of this work. First, we report a new chronometric analysis suggesting that increases in perceptual gain with speed stress are evident in FEF synaptic input, implicating afferent sensory-processing sources. Second, we report a new analysis demonstrating selective influence of SAT on frequency coupling between FEF neurons and local field potentials. None of these observations correspond to the mechanics of current accumulator models.

[1]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action , 2005, Neuron.

[3]  A. Osman,et al.  On the locus of speed-accuracy trade-off in reaction time: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[4]  Jeffrey D Schall,et al.  Nonhuman primate event-related potentials indexing covert shifts of attention , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  Partha P. Mitra,et al.  Sampling Properties of the Spectrum and Coherency of Sequences of Action Potentials , 2000, Neural Computation.

[6]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Dual diffusion model for single-cell recording data from the superior colliculus in a brightness-discrimination task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  H. Müller,et al.  Stimulus Saliency Modulates Pre-Attentive Processing Speed in Human Visual Cortex , 2011, PloS one.

[9]  Jiaxiang Zhang,et al.  The Effects of Evidence Bounds on Decision-Making: Theoretical and Empirical Developments , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[10]  G. Logan Shapes of reaction-time distributions and shapes of learning curves: a test of the instance theory of automaticity. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  Tirin Moore,et al.  Selection and Maintenance of Spatial Information by Frontal Eye Field Neurons , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural Activity in Macaque Parietal Cortex Reflects Temporal Integration of Visual Motion Signals during Perceptual Decision Making , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  R. Ratcliff A diffusion model account of response time and accuracy in a brightness discrimination task: Fitting real data and failing to fit fake but plausible data , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[16]  Gaby Maimon,et al.  Parietal Area 5 and the Initiation of Self-Timed Movements versus Simple Reactions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  D. P. Hanes,et al.  Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  C. Bruce,et al.  Frontal eye field efferents in the macaque monkey: II. Topography of terminal fields in midbrain and pons , 1988, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[19]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Cortico-cortical interactions in spatial attention: A combined ERP/TMS study. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Bogacz Optimal decision-making theories: linking neurobiology with behaviour , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  J. Gold,et al.  Caudate Encodes Multiple Computations for Perceptual Decisions , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  J. Schall,et al.  Visual and Motor Connectivity and the Distribution of Calcium-Binding Proteins in Macaque Frontal Eye Field: Implications for Saccade Target Selection , 2009, Front. Neuroanat..

[24]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neuronal Activity in the Lateral Intraparietal Area and Spatial Attention , 2003, Science.

[25]  Mounir Boukadoum,et al.  Mechanisms Gating the Flow of Information in the Cortex: What They Might Look Like and What Their Uses may be , 2010, Front. Comput. Neurosci..

[26]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[27]  Paul Sajda,et al.  Quality of evidence for perceptual decision making is indexed by trial-to-trial variability of the EEG , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[29]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[30]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Cortico-striatal connections predict control over speed and accuracy in perceptual decision making , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  P. Fries A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Braden A. Purcell,et al.  From Salience to Saccades: Multiple-Alternative Gated Stochastic Accumulator Model of Visual Search , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Desimone,et al.  Modulation of Oscillatory Neuronal Synchronization by Selective Visual Attention , 2001, Science.

[34]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Neural Correlates of Trial-to-Trial Fluctuations in Response Caution , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  J. Schall,et al.  Current advances and pressing problems in studies of stopping , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[36]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[37]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[38]  R. Desimone,et al.  Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection , 2006, Nature.

[39]  W. Singer,et al.  Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain , 2004, Nature.

[41]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of coherence differences , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[42]  Byounghoon Kim,et al.  Saccade Target Selection in the Superior Colliculus: A Signal Detection Theory Approach , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  The Neural and Computational Basis of Controlled Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff during Task Performance , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  Felix A. Wichmann,et al.  Rapid animal detection in natural scenes: Critical features are local , 2010 .

[45]  G. Woodman,et al.  The Effect of Visual Search Efficiency on Response Preparation , 2008, Psychological science.

[46]  R. Carpenter,et al.  The influence of urgency on decision time , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[48]  E. Miller,et al.  Response to Comment on "Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices" , 2007, Science.

[49]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[50]  A. Fuchs,et al.  The brainstem burst generator for saccadic eye movements , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  M. Bauer,et al.  Neural Characterization of the Speed–Accuracy Tradeoff in a Perceptual Decision-Making Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[53]  R. Marois,et al.  fMRI Evidence for a Dual Process Account of the Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff in Decision-Making , 2008, PloS one.

[54]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Relation of frontal eye field activity to saccade initiation during a countermanding task , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  R. Desimone,et al.  High-Frequency, Long-Range Coupling Between Prefrontal and Visual Cortex During Attention , 2009, Science.

[56]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Organization of monkey superior colliculus: enhanced visual response of superficial layer cells. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[58]  Robert Desimone,et al.  Parallel and Serial Neural Mechanisms for Visual Search in Macaque Area V4 , 2005, Science.

[59]  Kenway Louie,et al.  Separating Value from Choice: Delay Discounting Activity in the Lateral Intraparietal Area , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  Wayne A. Wickelgren,et al.  Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics , 1977 .

[61]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[62]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  Aditya Murthy,et al.  Neural control of visual search by frontal eye field: effects of unexpected target displacement on visual selection and saccade preparation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  Kaoru Yoshida,et al.  Saccade-related inhibitory input to pontine omnipause neurons: an intracellular study in alert cats. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  On the origin of event-related potentials indexing covert attentional selection during visual search. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  E. Keller,et al.  Colliculoreticular organization in primate oculomotor system. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Effects of Stimulus-Response Compatibility on Neural Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2003, Neuron.

[68]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neurally constrained modeling of perceptual decision making. , 2010, Psychological review.

[69]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[70]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[71]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification. , 1997, Psychological review.

[72]  A. Treisman,et al.  Conjunction search revisited. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[73]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[74]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[75]  H. Johnson,et al.  A comparison of 'traditional' and multimedia information systems development practices , 2003, Inf. Softw. Technol..

[76]  M. Eimer The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[77]  D P Munoz,et al.  Role of Primate Superior Colliculus in Preparation and Execution of Anti-Saccades and Pro-Saccades , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[78]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Reward Value-Based Gain Control: Divisive Normalization in Parietal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[79]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[80]  G. Karmos,et al.  Entrainment of Neuronal Oscillations as a Mechanism of Attentional Selection , 2008, Science.

[81]  D P Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[82]  M. Shadlen,et al.  The effect of stimulus strength on the speed and accuracy of a perceptual decision. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[83]  Thomas J. Palmeri,et al.  An Exemplar-Based Random Walk Model of Speeded Classification , 1997 .

[84]  J. Gold,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual decision making before, during, and after decision commitment in monkey frontal eye field. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[85]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neural basis of the set-size effect in frontal eye field: timing of attention during visual search. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[86]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[87]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision-making with multiple alternatives , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[88]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Executive control of gaze by the frontal lobes , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[89]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Frontal eye field activity before visual search errors reveals the integration of bottom-up and top-down salience. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[90]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[91]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff , 2012, Neuron.

[92]  Nikos K Logothetis,et al.  Interpreting the BOLD signal. , 2004, Annual review of physiology.

[93]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neural correlates of correct and errant attentional selection revealed through N2pc and frontal eye field activity. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[94]  S. Kelly,et al.  A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[95]  Ilya E. Monosov,et al.  Measurements of Simultaneously Recorded Spiking Activity and Local Field Potentials Suggest that Spatial Selection Emerges in the Frontal Eye Field , 2008, Neuron.

[96]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[97]  Kelly Shen,et al.  Investigating the role of the superior colliculus in active vision with the visual search paradigm , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[98]  M. Carandini,et al.  Local Origin of Field Potentials in Visual Cortex , 2009, Neuron.

[99]  L A Krubitzer,et al.  Frontal eye field as defined by intracortical microstimulation in squirrel monkeys, owl monkeys, and macaque monkeys II. cortical connections , 1986, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[100]  R. Bogacz,et al.  The neural basis of the speed–accuracy tradeoff , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.

[101]  Geoffrey F. Woodman,et al.  Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search , 1999, Nature.

[102]  Philip Holmes,et al.  Rapid decision threshold modulation by reward rate in a neural network , 2006, Neural Networks.

[103]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[104]  C. J. McGrath,et al.  Effect of exchange rate return on volatility spill-over across trading regions , 2012 .

[105]  Jay A. Hennig,et al.  Signal Multiplexing and Single-Neuron Computations in Lateral Intraparietal Area During Decision-Making , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[106]  C. Olson,et al.  In Monkeys Making Value-Based Decisions, LIP Neurons Encode Cue Salience and Not Action Value , 2012, Science.

[107]  J. Assad,et al.  A cognitive signal for the proactive timing of action in macaque LIP , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[108]  M. Segraves Activity of monkey frontal eye field neurons projecting to oculomotor regions of the pons. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[109]  C. Bundesen,et al.  A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology. , 2005, Psychological review.

[110]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of the Set-Size Effect in Monkey Lateral Intraparietal Area , 2008, PLoS biology.

[111]  B A J Reddi,et al.  Accuracy, information, and response time in a saccadic decision task. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[112]  S. Luck,et al.  Neural sources of focused attention in visual search. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.