Pamela WoolnerSchool of Education, Communication and Language Sciences University of Newcastle Upon Tyne Newcastle Upon Tyne NE1 7RU UKDespite mathematicians valuing the ability to visualize a problem and psychologists finding positive correlations of visual-spatial ability with success in mathematics, many educationists remain unconvinced about the benefits of visualization for mathematical understanding. This paper describes research that compared a ‘visual’ to a ‘verbal’ teaching approach through teaching a range of early secondary school mathematics topics to two classes using one or other approach. The two classes were compared through a post-intervention test of mathematical competency, on which the verbally taught class scored significantly higher. No interactions were found between teaching style and the learner’s preferred style although the pupils identified as ‘visualizers’ did tend to perform more poorly. INTRODUCTIONUnderstanding mathematics, visually and verbally Educators would tend to agree that a major aim of teaching mathematics is for students to develop understanding, even if it can be difficult to conceptualize this understanding fully (Sfard, 1994: Sierpinska, 1994). Taking a broadly constructivist view of knowledge, as teachers are inclined to (Sfard, 1994), suggests the importance of individually constructed understanding. Yet this interpretation still leaves the problem of how teachers interact with learners’ constructions and facilitate their building. The challenge of communication can lead beyond a concern with issues of vocabulary and clarity to a tendency to think that verbal descriptions constitute knowledge (Davis, 1984) and that mathematical abstraction is essentially verbal (Anghileri, 1999). However, the history of the development of mathematical concepts points to the importance within the subject of its visual side (Sfard, 1991). Furthermore, it has been argued that students should be encouraged to develop this aspect within their own understanding (Davis, 1984). It seems necessary to see how a broadly visual approach compares in the classroom to a more verbal approach.
[1]
Alan D. Baddeley,et al.
Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition
,
1990
.
[2]
P. E. Vernon.
Mathematics Competency Test: User's Manual.
,
1996
.
[3]
A. Paivio.
Imagery and verbal processes
,
1972
.
[4]
J. Richardson.
Mental imagery and human memory
,
1980
.
[5]
P. Johnson-Laird,et al.
Visual imagery can impede reasoning
,
2002,
Memory & cognition.
[6]
Norma C. Presmeg,et al.
Visualisation and mathematical giftedness
,
1986
.
[7]
A. Baddeley.
Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition
,
1990
.
[8]
A. Sfard.
On the dual nature of mathematical conceptions: Reflections on processes and objects as different sides of the same coin
,
1991
.
[9]
Anna Sierpinska,et al.
Understanding in Mathematics
,
1994
.
[10]
A. Sfard.
Reification as the birth of metaphor
,
1994
.
[11]
Despina A. Stylianou,et al.
On the interaction of visualization and analysis: the negotiation of a visual representation in expert problem solving
,
2002
.
[12]
V. A. Krutetskii.
The psychology of mathematical abilities in shoolchildren / V.A. Krutetskii
,
1988
.
[13]
Norma C. Presmeg,et al.
Prototypes, metaphors, metonymies and imaginative rationality in high school mathematics
,
1992
.
[14]
Martin Hughes,et al.
Issues in Teaching Numeracy in Primary Schools
,
1999
.
[15]
Robert B. Davis.
Learning Mathematics: The Cognitive Science Approach to Mathematics Education
,
1984
.
[16]
Mary Hegarty,et al.
Revising the Visualizer-Verbalizer Dimension: Evidence for Two Types of Visualizers
,
2002
.