The development of the spatial extent of oculomotor inhibition

Inhibition is intimately involved in the ability to select a target for a goal-directed movement. The effect of distracters on the deviation of oculomotor trajectories and landing positions provides evidence of such inhibition. Individual saccade trajectories and landing positions may deviate initially either towards, or away from, a competing distracter--the direction and extent of this deviation depends upon saccade latency and the target to distracter separation. However, the underlying commonality of the sources of oculomotor inhibition has not been investigated. Here we report the relationship between distracter-related deviation of saccade trajectory, landing position and saccade latency. Observers saccaded to a target which could be accompanied by a distracter shown at various distances from very close (10 angular degrees) to far away (120 angular degrees). A fixation-gap paradigm was used to manipulate latency independently of the influence of competing distracters. When distracters were close to the target, saccade trajectory and landing position deviated toward the distracter position, while at greater separations landing position was always accurate but trajectories deviated away from the distracters. Different spatial patterns of deviations across latency were found. This pattern of results is consistent with the metrics of the saccade reflecting coarse pooling of the ongoing activity at the distracter location: saccade trajectory reflects activity at saccade initiation while landing position reveals activity at saccade end.

[1]  S. Tipper,et al.  Reaching affects saccade trajectories , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  D. F. Fisher,et al.  Eye movements : cognition and visual perception , 1982 .

[3]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Reversible inactivation of monkey superior colliculus. I. Curvature of saccadic trajectory. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[5]  D. Munoz,et al.  Competitive Integration of Visual and Preparatory Signals in the Superior Colliculus during Saccadic Programming , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  P. Haggard,et al.  Time course of oculomotor inhibition revealed by saccade trajectory modulation. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  H. Deubel,et al.  Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  E. McSorley,et al.  Involuntary inhibition of movement initiation alters oculomotor competition resolution , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[10]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[11]  S M Ross,et al.  Saccade latency and warning signals: Stimulus onset, offset, and change as warning events , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  L. Optican,et al.  Model of the control of saccades by superior colliculus and cerebellum. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sequential activity of simultaneously recorded neurons in the superior colliculus during curved saccades. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  P. Haggard,et al.  The control of saccade trajectories: Direction of curvature depends on prior knowledge of target location and saccade latency , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  D. Munoz,et al.  Lateral interactions in the superior colliculus, not an extended fixation zone, can account for the remote distractor effect , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  F. Ottes,et al.  Latency dependence of colour-based target vs nontarget discrimination by the saccadic system , 1985, Vision Research.

[17]  A. Opstal,et al.  A nonlinear model for collicular spatial interactions underlying the metrical properties of electrically elicited saccades , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[18]  Digby Elliott,et al.  The effects of response priming on the planning and execution of goal-directed movements in the presence of a distracting stimulus. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[19]  A. Allport Attention and control: have we been asking the wrong questions? A critical review of twenty-five years , 1993 .

[20]  J T McIlwain,et al.  Distributed spatial coding in the superior colliculus: A review , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.

[21]  Casimir J. H. Ludwig,et al.  Target similarity affects saccade curvature away from irrelevant onsets , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Eugene McSorley,et al.  Distractor modulation of saccade trajectories: spatial separation and symmetry effects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Neeraj J Gandhi,et al.  Simulations of saccade curvature by models that place superior colliculus upstream from the local feedback loop. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Casimir J. H. Ludwig,et al.  Measuring saccade curvature: A curve-fitting approach , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[25]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Incomplete Suppression of Distractor-Related Activity in the Frontal Eye Field Results in Curved Saccades , 2006 .

[26]  P. H. Schiller,et al.  Express averaging saccades in monkeys , 1999, Vision Research.

[27]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Eugene McSorley,et al.  Saccade target selection in visual search: accuracy improves when more distractors are present. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[29]  D. Munoz,et al.  Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  P. Haggard,et al.  Spatial and temporal aspects of oculomotor inhibition as revealed by saccade trajectories , 2005, Vision Research.

[31]  R. McPeek,et al.  Target selection for visually guided reaching in macaque. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  J. Tanji Sequential organization of multiple movements: involvement of cortical motor areas. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  E. Keller,et al.  Short-term priming, concurrent processing, and saccade curvature during a target selection task in the monkey , 2001, Vision Research.

[35]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Relation between saccade trajectories and spatial distractor locations. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[36]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[37]  Robert M McPeek,et al.  Competition between saccade goals in the superior colliculus produces saccade curvature. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Kuniharu Arai,et al.  A model of the saccade-generating system that accounts for trajectory variations produced by competing visual stimuli , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[39]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Independent contributions of the orienting of attention, fixation offset and bilateral stimulation on human saccadic latencies , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The spatial coding of the inhibition evoked by distractors , 2007, Vision Research.

[41]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Representation of averaging saccades in the superior colliculus of the monkey , 1993, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Valerie Brown,et al.  Eye scanning of multi-element displays: II. Saccade planning , 2006, Vision Research.

[43]  J. Findlay Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements , 1982, Vision Research.